Outline
Comptes Rendus

Neurosciences
Altered processing of sensorimotor feedback in schizophrenia
Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 330 (2007) no. 5, pp. 382-388.

Abstracts

Schizophrenia is characterized by an altered sense of the reality, associated with hallucinations and delusions. Some theories suggest that schizophrenia is related to a deficiency of the system that generates information about the sensory consequences of the actions realized by the subject. This system monitors the reafferent information resulting from an action and allows its anticipation. In the present study, we examined visual-event-related potentials (ERPs) generated by a sensorimotor task in 15 patients with schizophrenia and 15 normal controls. The visual feedback from hand movements performed by the subjects was experimentally distorted. Behavioral results showed that patients were impaired in recognizing their own movements. The ERP signal in patients also differed from those of control subjects. In patients, the ERP waveform was affected during the early part of the response (200 ms). This early effect in schizophrenic patients reveals a modified processing of the visual consequence of their actions.

La schizophrénie est caractérisée par des hallucinations et des idées délirantes quant à l'attribution des propres mouvements du sujet, ce qui peut s'expliquer par un déficit de l'anticipation des conséquences de leurs actions. Les potentiels évoqués en réponse à l'exécution d'un mouvement ont été enregistrés chez 15 schizophrènes et 15 sujets normaux. Le feedback visuel résultant des mouvements exécutés par ces sujets a été modifié au cours d'essais successifs, de telle sorte que les sujets voyaient une image de leurs mouvements déviée par rapport à leur direction réelle. La déviation pouvait être de 0°, 15°, 30° et 60°. Les sujets devaient répondre, par oui ou par non, à la question de savoir si l'image qu'ils voyaient correspondait au mouvement qu'ils avaient exécuté. Les résultats comportementaux ont confirmé des travaux antérieurs utilisant la même technique, en ce sens que les malades schizophrènes reconnaissent moins bien leurs propres mouvements que les sujets normaux. Les sujets normaux répondaient au hasard (50% de réponses OUI) pour une déviation angulaire de 15°. Les schizophrènes n'atteignaient cette valeur de 50% qu'entre 30° et 60°. Les potentiels évoqués ont montré des différences entre les essais, avec différents degrés de déviation dans des fenêtres temporelles différentes chez les sujets normaux et chez les malades. Chez les sujets normaux, la modulation du potentiel évoqué en fonction de la déviation était présente dans les composantes tardives de la réponse (500–900 ms), ce qui plaide en faveur d'une modulation du traitement cognitif du stimulus. Chez les malades, cette modulation était absente dans la composante tardive, mais était en revanche très marquée dans une fenêtre temporelle précoce (100–300 ms). Ces résultats suggèrent que le trouble de la reconnaissance des actions chez le malade schizophrène pourrait être dû à une altération du traitement précoce du stimulus visuel, celui qui permet la reconnaissance du stimulus et non son traitement cognitif.

Metadata
Received:
Accepted:
Published online:
DOI: 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003
Keywords: Psychiatry, EEG, Cognitive processes, Visuomotor integration, Clinical neurophysiology
Mots-clés : Psychiatrie, EEG, Processus cognitifs, Intégration visuomotrice, Neurophysiologie clinique

Andrés Posada 1, 2; Nicolas Franck 1, 3; Ségolène Augier 1; Nicolas Georgieff 1, 3; Marc Jeannerod 1

1 Institut des sciences cognitives, 67, bd Pinel, 69675 Bron, France
2 Centro Internacional de Fisica, Apartado Aéreo 49490, Bogotá, Colombia
3 Centre hospitalier Le-Vinatier, 95, bd Pinel, 69677 Bron cedex, France
@article{CRBIOL_2007__330_5_382_0,
     author = {Andr\'es Posada and Nicolas Franck and S\'egol\`ene Augier and Nicolas Georgieff and Marc Jeannerod},
     title = {Altered processing of sensorimotor feedback in schizophrenia},
     journal = {Comptes Rendus. Biologies},
     pages = {382--388},
     publisher = {Elsevier},
     volume = {330},
     number = {5},
     year = {2007},
     doi = {10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003},
     language = {en},
}
TY  - JOUR
AU  - Andrés Posada
AU  - Nicolas Franck
AU  - Ségolène Augier
AU  - Nicolas Georgieff
AU  - Marc Jeannerod
TI  - Altered processing of sensorimotor feedback in schizophrenia
JO  - Comptes Rendus. Biologies
PY  - 2007
SP  - 382
EP  - 388
VL  - 330
IS  - 5
PB  - Elsevier
DO  - 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003
LA  - en
ID  - CRBIOL_2007__330_5_382_0
ER  - 
%0 Journal Article
%A Andrés Posada
%A Nicolas Franck
%A Ségolène Augier
%A Nicolas Georgieff
%A Marc Jeannerod
%T Altered processing of sensorimotor feedback in schizophrenia
%J Comptes Rendus. Biologies
%D 2007
%P 382-388
%V 330
%N 5
%I Elsevier
%R 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003
%G en
%F CRBIOL_2007__330_5_382_0
Andrés Posada; Nicolas Franck; Ségolène Augier; Nicolas Georgieff; Marc Jeannerod. Altered processing of sensorimotor feedback in schizophrenia. Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 330 (2007) no. 5, pp. 382-388. doi : 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2007.02.003/

Original version of the full text

1 Introduction

Schizophrenia is a complex and potentially severe mental illness, characterized by its heterogeneous clinical presentation [1]. Several authors attempted to isolate an essential trouble that may underlie its main symptoms. In an assay to facilitate diagnosis, Kurt Schneider [2] defined first-rank symptoms that he claimed were specific to schizophrenia. The first-rank symptoms relate to an abnormal recognition of one's own action, and an abnormal attribution of one's actions to others (see [3] for review). Patients describe their symptoms as experiences consecutive to the influence of alien forces or other people on their own thoughts (made thoughts, thought broadcasting, withdrawal, or insertion), actions (made actions) or emotions (made feelings). Other symptoms are described as very extraneous experiences, like auditory verbal hallucinations, e.g., sentences that seem to be pronounced around the subject.

This impaired attribution of action has been interpreted as an abnormal matching between information related to voluntarily generated actions and their sensory consequences [4]. This effect could be caused by a deterioration of the forward model [5,6] responsible for the prediction of the consequences of voluntary acts. Numerous studies have evaluated the capacity of action attribution of patients with schizophrenia [7–9]. In general, they used experimental paradigms based on the alteration of the sensory feedback associated with voluntary actions (hand gesture or language) to determine whether subjects were able to recognize their own actions. Globally, patients with schizophrenia show a reduced accuracy in discriminating between their own action and a foreign, or modified, action [10,11].

Neuroimaging studies in control subjects have shown that the activity of the inferior part of the right parietal lobe is modulated by the degree of discrepancy between an executed movement and its observed consequences: the larger the discrepancy, the greater the activation [12]. In schizophrenic patients, the activity in this same parietal area is poorly modulated by the degree of discrepancy [13]. These studies, although they provide valuable information about brain areas involved in action recognition, fail to describe the chronology of brain events. This aspect is particularly important, since an abnormal time course of events could account for the main symptoms that have been described in schizophrenic patients [14]. The Event-Related Potential (ERP) technique is an adequate method for the study of the temporal aspects of cognitive processes, due to its high temporal resolution. In the context of action recognition, it can provide a physiological marker associated with the processing of the sensory consequence of a voluntary movement.

We carried out an ERP experiment in 15 patients with schizophrenia and 15 normal subjects using a task designed to evaluate action recognition and attribution [9]. In this task, subjects moved a joystick while observing a virtual image of their hand holding a joystick. The movement of the virtual image could correspond, or not, to the real movement. We measured ERP associated with the display of the virtual image corresponding to the end-result of the movements made by the subjects.

2 Material and methods

2.1 Subjects

Fifteen right-handed patients (13 males, 2 females) diagnosed with schizophrenia according to DSM-IV were recruited from the Le-Vinatier Hospital in Lyons, France. They had a mean age of 32.7±10 years. All were exempted of any neurological antecedent. Their clinical status was evaluated with the Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms SAPS [15] and the Scale for Assessment Negative Symptoms SANS [16]. The mean total SAPS and SANS scores (±SD) were respectively 30.9 (24.2) and 38.3 (21.2). The mean duration of illness was 9.4±9.2 years. All patients were under stable second-generation antipsychotic treatment during the experiment.

Fifteen right-handed subjects (8 males and 7 females) with a mean age of age of 26.8 years±6.3 without psychiatric and neurological antecedents have been included. They formed the control group.

This experiment was approved by the local ethical committee.

2.2 Stimuli and task

A realistic image of a virtual right hand holding a joystick was generated on a computer using specific software. Simultaneously, the subject held a real joystick fixed on a table, 66 cm away from the screen. The real hand was hidden by a black cloth so that the subjects could only see the movement of the virtual hand on the screen. In normal trials, the virtual image exactly followed the hand movements made by the subjects. A variable angular bias could be introduced electronically between the movements of the real hand and those of the virtual hand.

The experiment included 160 trials. For every trial, the task consisted to perform a straight movement of the joystick in any direction (free choice of the subject) after the display of a yellow cross in the centre of the screen. Subjects were instructed to maintain the position at the end of the movement and to avoid returning to the starting position of the joystick. 500 ms after the end of the movement, the image of the virtual hand holding the joystick was displayed. This image remained for 2000 ms and was replaced by a screen with the question: “Did the picture that you just saw correspond to the end of the movement you made?”; the subject responded verbally by yes or no whether the image corresponded or not to the movement. The verbal response was recorded by the experimenter (see Fig. 1 for a description of the protocol). Trials involved either no bias (0 degree) or biases of 15, 30, or 60 degrees. Forty trials in each condition were used. The order of presentation was pseudo-random; the same bias was never repeated more than two times in a row.

Fig. 1

Temporal description of the different events succeeding in one trial of the task.

2.3 EEG recording

Scalp voltage was collected with 65 channels Geodesic Sensor Net through AC coupled high-input-impedance amplifiers (200 MΩ, Net Amps, Electrical Geodesics Inc., Eugene, OR, USA). The net had silver/silver chloride (Ag/AgCl) electrodes. Amplified analogue voltages (0.1–200 Hz bandwidth) were sampled at 500 Hz. The electrode impedance was kept below 50 kΩ. An electrode placed in the neck and another one placed in the vertex served as ground and record reference, respectively.

Trials contaminated by eye blinks or eye movements were rejected offline from the analysis using an algorithm developed by Electrical Geodesic Inc, which detects fast and high voltage variations. The same algorithm, but with lower thresholds, was also used to remove artefacts. We used a criterion of rejection for all segments with a voltage higher than ±100 μV. All channels with more than 50 percent of artefacts were removed from the analysis. In total, 20% of the segments were eliminated in the patients group and 14% in the control group.

2.4 EEG analysis

EEG records were segmented aligned to the display of the image of the virtual hand showing the end-result of the movement. ERPs of each bias were calculated by averaging the segments. A baseline correction was applied by subtracting the average value of the 200 ms segment before the display of the stimulus. The ERPs of the 65 channels were averaged in nine symmetrical regions on the scalp. For the analysis of the different ERP components, we averaged the waveform using three temporal windows: 100–300 ms, 300–500 ms, and 500–900 ms.

The statistical analysis was carried on with a four-level repeated measures ANOVA Group (patients, controls) × bias (four modes: 0, 15, 30, and 60 degrees) × Ant-Post (anterior, central, posterior) × Laterality (left, middle, right) for each temporal window. The Huynh–Feldt correction was applied for the repeated measures of the ANOVA analysis.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioural data

The percentage of yes responses for each value of bias is presented in Fig. 2. In each group of subjects, the percentages of yes responses decreased when the angular bias was increased. However, the two groups differed in the value of the bias for which the subjects responded at a chance level (50% yes responses). In the control group, the chance level approximately corresponded to a bias of 15°, whereas in the patients group, the chance level was attained for larger biases (beyond 30°).

Fig. 2

Percentage of yes responses for each value of the angular bias in the patient and the control groups.

A statistical analysis was carried on with a two-level repeated-measure ANOVA group (patients, controls) × bias (0°, 15°, 30°, 60°). The analysis of variance revealed a group effect (F(1,28)=8.16, p=0.008), a bias effect (F(1.64,84)=53.8, p<0.0001), but no significant interaction group × bias (F(1.64,84)=2.8, p=0.076). However, a direct analysis (1 factor ANOVA) clearly revealed that the proportion of yes responses significantly differed between the two groups for the 15° bias (p=0.025) and the 30° bias (p=0.001).

3.2 ERP data

Figs. 3B–C and 4B–C show the pattern of ERPs obtained in control and patient groups in the posterior–middle and the central–middle regions of the scalp, respectively. Although the global pattern of the responses appears to be relatively similar in the two groups, significant differences between groups were revealed by the statistical analysis of the waveform of responses to the different values of the bias, according to the three time windows.

Fig. 3

Example of the ERP activity in the posterior–middle region of the scalp. (A) Scalp location of the averaged electrodes. (BC) ERP waveform for each bias in the control and patients group; 0 corresponds to the visual feedback display. (D) Histogram with the values of the ERP amplitude averaged in the 100–300-ms time window for each bias in each group.

Fig. 4

Example of the ERP activity in the central–middle region of the scalp. (A) Scalp location of the averaged electrodes. (BC) ERP waveform for each bias in the control and patients group. (D) Histogram with the values of the ERP amplitude averaged in the 500–900-ms time window for each bias in each group.

The statistical analysis of the average ERP voltage in the 100–300-ms time window (Fig. 3D) revealed an interaction group × bias (F(2.95,84)=3.7, p=0.016) and an interaction group × bias × antero-posterior (F(3.5,168)=3.3, p=0.018). The post-hoc analysis showed ERP waveform differences in the patient group, between the 0° and the 30° biases (p=0.0003), between the 15° and the 30° biases (p=0.01), and between the 30° and the 60° biases (p=0.002), especially in the posterior region of the scalp. No such differences were found for this time window in the control group.

Similar results were found in the 300–500-ms time window. The same interaction group × bias × antero-posterior was present (F(3.9,168)=3.501, p=0.011). Again, the post-hoc analysis showed ERP waveform differences in the patient group between 0°–30° biases (p=0.03), but this time in the central region of the scalp.

Different results were found in statistical analysis of ERP waveforms in the late time window (500–900 ms) (Fig. 4D). The ANOVA also revealed an interaction group × bias × antero-posterior (F(3.66,168)=2.76, p=0.036), but the post-hoc analysis revealed different effects. Post-hoc comparisons showed differences in the control group only, between the 0° and the 15° biases (p=0.02), the 15° and the 60° biases (p=0.01) and the 30° and the 60° biases (p=0.03) in the central region of the scalp.

4 Discussion

In this study, we measured the brain activity (using the ERP technique) associated with the recognition of one's own movements in patients with schizophrenia. The subjects had to identify (by a yes/no response) whether the end-result they saw corresponded to the movement they had made. A stepwise distortion of the visual feedback of their movement was produced by introducing angular biases. The proportion of 50% of yes responses represents the level of discrepancy at which the subject could not determine whether the end-result corresponded, or not, to his/her movement, and responded at chance. In terms of the ability to attribute correctly a movement to its author, the 50% value may therefore be considered as the threshold that separates a self-attributed movement from a movement perceived as originating from another agent. According to the behavioural data recorded in the present experiment, this threshold value was found to be higher in patients (beyond the 30° bias) than in control subjects (around the 15° bias). This result confirms previous results by Franck et al. [9].

The main result of the present study was that the ERP waveform recorded during identification of the end-result of a movement was modified according to the threshold values in each group of subjects. This modification, however, was observed in different time windows for each group. Let us first examine the pattern of ERPs in control and patient groups for the late time window (500–900 ms). In subjects of the control group, a clear and significant difference was observed between the ERPs obtained during the ‘unambiguous’ trials, i.e., those trials with no bias (0°) or with a large bias (60°), where the vast majority of responses were either yes or no, and the ‘ambiguous’ trials (15°), where responses were at chance. The late ERP component corresponding to these ‘ambiguous’ trials showed increased amplitude in comparison with the ‘unambiguous’ ones. The late ERP components are usually associated with the manipulation of the cognitive parameters of the stimulus (for example, introducing syntactic errors in a sentence [17]). It may be suggested that, in the present situation, the difficulty to evaluate the difference between the visual feedback and the movement during ‘ambiguous’ trials yielded, in the control group, a deeper cognitive analysis of the sensory information, which resulted in an increase of the ERP activity. This was not present in the patient group. Note that the absence of modulation of the late ERP components by cognitive manipulations is a common finding in schizophrenic patients (e.g., unlike normal controls, they do not show a N400 component for unexpected words or mismatch negativity for unexpected auditory stimuli [18–20]).

The ERP waveform of the patients also showed a modulation according to the degree of bias, but in a time window (around 200 ms) earlier than that of the control group. The ERP amplitude was larger for the 30° bias, in comparison to the others. As in the control group, this corresponds to the ‘ambiguous’ trials, those for which the patients responded at chance. In the ERP literature, the early components reflect processing of stimulus characteristics, rather than its cognitive content. For example, the N170 component (latency 170 ms) is associated with the processing of face images [21]. Similarly, the burst of EEG oscillations at the gamma frequency appearing 280 ms after the display of a stimulus is associated with the elaboration of a final coherent representation of the stimulus [22], and the integrated representation can be manipulated by cognitive processes only after this step [23].

The fact that in the schizophrenic patients, the ERP change triggered by the sensorimotor conflict was observed in a time window that is generally considered as corresponding to the sensory stage of processing, contrary to the control subjects who showed a change in their later, ‘cognitive’ window, is a novel and interesting finding. This result suggests an impaired processing of the basic aspects of a visual stimulus, i.e., those aspects that are critical for shape recognition. Having not properly analyzed the visual display of their hand position, they would not notice the discrepancy between this position and their movement until the mismatch becomes very large. This interpretation raises the question of a possible role of the antipsychotic drugs that the patients received at the time of the experiment, in perturbing their visual sensory processing. This role is unlikely, however, if one considers the fact that the difficulty of schizophrenic patients in recognizing their own movements is present (and even worse) at the acute stage of their disease, i.e., before they have received any treatment. It seems more reasonable to assume that the deficit in action recognition observed in our patients, and the concomitant changes in their ERP responses, is a genuine aspect of their pathology.

The significance of the present results showing an altered ERP response to a sensorimotor conflict in schizophrenic patients is still a matter of speculation. Sensorimotor conflicts like the one used in the present study are known to modulate the activity of the posterior parietal cortex [12,24]. As we saw in the Introduction, according to Farrer et al. [12], posterior parietal activity covariates with the degree of conflict, being higher when the degree of conflict increases. Interestingly, this modulation tends to disappear in schizophrenic patients [13]. The relation between these two facts (the change in parietal cortex activity and the change in ERP responses during a sensorimotor conflict) can be interpreted in two ways. According to one interpretation, the lack of modulation of parietal cortex activity can be seen as the consequence of the impaired early visual processing. Alternatively, the change in parietal cortex activity can be seen as the primary factor, the impaired ERP response being a consequence of a perturbed top–down modulation of visual cortex by parietal cortex. In support to the second interpretation is the general idea of a disruption of frontal cortex activity in schizophrenia (see review in [25]), with the correlative disinhibition of more posterior cortical areas, including posterior parietal cortex. Whichever is the correct one, the two interpretations would equally well account for the patients difficulty in recognizing their own movements.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the CNRS and the ‘Université Claude-Bernard’, Lyons, France. Funding was also provided by the European Union (Fifth Framework Program, Proposal No. QLRT-2001–00746). We thank Prof. Michel Marie-Cardine for his crucial help for the evaluation of the patients.


References

[1] N.C. Andreasen Symptoms, signs, and diagnosis of schizophrenia, Lancet, Volume 19 (1995), pp. 477-481

[2] K. Schneider Klinische Psychopathologie, Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart, Germany, 1955

[3] M. Jeannerod; C. Farrer; N. Franck; P. Fourneret; E. Daprati; N. Georgieff Action recognition in normal and schizophrenic subjects (T. Kircher; A. Davis, eds.), The Self in Neuroscience and Psychiatry, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2003, pp. 380-406

[4] C.D. Frith The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove, 1992

[5] C.D. Frith; S.J. Blakemore; D.M. Wolpert Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action, Brain Res. Rev., Volume 31 (2003), pp. 57-63

[6] D.M. Wolpert; Z. Ghahramani; M.I. Jordan An internal model for sensorimotor integration, Science, Volume 29 (1995), pp. 1880-1882

[7] E. Daprati; N. Franck; N. Georgieff; J. Proust; E. Pacherie; J. Dalery; M.-P. Jeannerod Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients, Cognition, Volume 65 (1997), pp. 71-86

[8] L.C. Johns; P.K. McGuire Verbal self-monitoring and auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia, Lancet, Volume 353 (1999), pp. 469-470

[9] N. Franck; C. Farrer; N. Georgieff; M. Marie-Cardine; J. Dalery; T. d'Amato; M. Jeannerod Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia, Am. J. Psychiatry, Volume 158 (2001), pp. 454-459

[10] S.S. Shergill; G. Samson; P.M. Bays; C.D. Frith; D.M. Wolpert Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia, Am. J. Psychiatry, Volume 162 (2005), pp. 2384-2386

[11] A. Lindner; P. Their; T.T. Kirchner; T. Haarmeier; D.T. Leube Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions, Curr. Biol., Volume 15 (2005), pp. 1119-1124

[12] C. Farrer; N. Franck; N. Georgieff; C.D. Frith; J. Decety; M. Jeannerod Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study, Neuroimage, Volume 18 (2003), pp. 324-333

[13] C. Farrer; N. Franck; C.D. Frith; J. Decety; N. Georgieff; T. d'Amato; M. Jeannerod Neural correlates of action attribution in schizophrenia, Psychiatry Res., Volume 131 (2004), pp. 31-44

[14] S.A. Spence Are first-rank symptoms encryption errors?, Br. J. Psychiatry, Volume 73 (1998), pp. 184-185

[15] N.C. Andreasen The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS), The University of Iowa, Iowa City, 1984

[16] N.C. Andreasen The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS), The University of Iowa, Iowa City, 1983

[17] A.D. Friederici; S.A. Kotz The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional imaging and lesion studies, Neuroimage, Volume 20 (2003), p. S8-S17

[18] A. Posada; E. Hugues; N. Franck; P. Vianin; J. Kilner Augmentation of induced visual gamma activity by increased task complexity, Eur. J. Neurosci., Volume 18 (2003), pp. 2351-2356

[19] N. Kumar; J.-B. Debruille Semantics and N400: insights for schizophrenia, J. Psychiatry Neurosci., Volume 29 (2004), pp. 89-98

[20] D. Umbricht; R. Koller; L. Schmid; A. Skrabo; C. Grubel; T. Huber; H. Stassen How specific are deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia?, Biol. Psychiatry, Volume 53 (2003), pp. 1120-1131

[21] B. Jemel; A.M. Schuller; Y. Cheref-Khan; V. Goffaux; M. Crommelinck; R. Bruyer Stepwise emergence of the face-sensitive N170 event-related potential component, Neuroreport, Volume 14 (2003), pp. 2035-2039

[22] C. Tallon-Baudry; O. Bertrand Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation, Trends Cognitive Sci., Volume 3 (1999), pp. 151-162

[23] J.M. Ford; D.H. Mathalon Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking, J. Psychiatr. Res., Volume 38 (2004), pp. 37-46

[24] G.R. Fink; J.C. Marshall; P.W. Halligan; C.D. Frith; J. Driver; R.S. Frackowiak; R.J. Dolan The neural consequences of conflict between intention and the senses, Brain, Volume 122 (1999), pp. 497-512

[25] D.R. Weinberger; K.F. Berman Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: confounds and controversies, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, Volume 351 (1996), pp. 1495-1503


Comments - Policy