Plan
Comptes Rendus

Ethology / Éthologie
Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli)
[Similarités vocales et relations sociales chez la mone de Campbell (Cercopithecus campbelli)]
Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 326 (2003) no. 12, pp. 1185-1193.

Résumés

This study examined variations of social call structures in female Campbell's monkeys. The comparison of frequency modulations using a similarity index revealed that each individual presented one to three variants. Some variants were shared by two to several individuals, often associated with play. On the contrary, vocal divergence was observed in a socially isolated animal and a negative correlation appeared between similarity index and the frequency of avoidance. This preliminary study gives the first evidence of variant sharing between group members in primates.

Cette étude examine les variations structurales des cris sociaux de mones de Campbell. La comparaison des modulations de fréquence, basée sur un indice de similarité, a révélé que chaque individu présentait une à trois variantes, dont certaines partagées avec un ou plusieurs individus, essentiellement des partenaires de jeu. Au contraire, une divergence vocale a été observée chez un animal socialement isolé et une corrélation négative est apparue entre la valeur de l'indice et la fréquence des évitements entre femelles. Cette étude préliminaire est la première mise en évidence d'un partage vocal entre membres d'un groupe de primates.

Métadonnées
Reçu le :
Accepté le :
Publié le :
DOI : 10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005
Keywords: vocal similarity, social bonds, guenon
Mot clés : similarité vocale, liens sociaux, guenon
Alban Lemasson 1 ; Jean-Pierre Gautier 1 ; Martine Hausberger 1

1 UMR 6552–CNRS, Éthologie–Évolution–Écologie, université Rennes-1, campus de Beaulieu, 35042 Rennes cedex, France
@article{CRBIOL_2003__326_12_1185_0,
     author = {Alban Lemasson and Jean-Pierre Gautier and Martine Hausberger},
     title = {Vocal similarities and social bonds in {Campbell's} monkey {(\protect\emph{Cercopithecus} campbelli})},
     journal = {Comptes Rendus. Biologies},
     pages = {1185--1193},
     publisher = {Elsevier},
     volume = {326},
     number = {12},
     year = {2003},
     doi = {10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005},
     language = {en},
}
TY  - JOUR
AU  - Alban Lemasson
AU  - Jean-Pierre Gautier
AU  - Martine Hausberger
TI  - Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli)
JO  - Comptes Rendus. Biologies
PY  - 2003
SP  - 1185
EP  - 1193
VL  - 326
IS  - 12
PB  - Elsevier
DO  - 10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005
LA  - en
ID  - CRBIOL_2003__326_12_1185_0
ER  - 
%0 Journal Article
%A Alban Lemasson
%A Jean-Pierre Gautier
%A Martine Hausberger
%T Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli)
%J Comptes Rendus. Biologies
%D 2003
%P 1185-1193
%V 326
%N 12
%I Elsevier
%R 10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005
%G en
%F CRBIOL_2003__326_12_1185_0
Alban Lemasson; Jean-Pierre Gautier; Martine Hausberger. Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli). Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 326 (2003) no. 12, pp. 1185-1193. doi : 10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005/

Version originale du texte intégral

1 Introduction

Vocal sharing or convergence has been shown to reflect social affinities in several species of birds (Australian magpie [1]; indigo buntings [2]; nightingales [3]) and cetaceans (bottlenose dolphins [4,5]; killer whales [6,7]). In European starlings [8,9] and male bottlenose dolphins [10,11], vocal sharing reflects spatial associations between social partners. Both learning common structures and structural convergence are implied in all these species. Forced grouping of unfamiliar budgerigars induced convergence in contact calls [1,12].

Social influence on call usage is well known in primates [13–17]. Although flexibility of structures and production learning capacities have been described in details in many species at lower phylogenetical levels (birds: review in [18]; cetaceans: [5,11,19–21]), vocal signals are thought to be fixed at birth in primates [13,22,23]. Nevertheless, studies of chimpanzees and Japanese macaques have illustrated variability in call production related to dialects [24,25], vocal adjustment [26,27] or conditioning by caregivers [28]. More recently, trill plasticity in adult pygmy marmosets was evidenced after changes in the social environment and during the development of infant calls [29–31].

Snowdon suggested that, if flexibility is possible, (i) it is more likely to occur in affiliative vocalizations and (ii) it may be very subtle and require the use of precise measurement methods [30]. Our first step in a long-term study on vocal communication and social organization in Campbell's monkeys, was to test the hypothesis that, as found in birds and cetaceans, the vocal structures of preferential social partners would be more similar than those of non-associated group members. We focused on calls known for their implication in social interactions (cohesion–contact calls [32]). To compare frequency modulations we used an index of similarity derived from those used successfully for cetaceans and birds [6,33–35].

2 Methods

2.1 Subjects and housing conditions

The group studied included one adult male (Gavroche, 12.5 years old) and the members of two matrilines: matriline 1 (mother: Lisa 14.5; daughters: Plume 6.5, Lowina 5.5, Maricopa 4, Chilula 3) and matriline 2 (sisters: Shawnee 5.5, Tilamook 3, Bela 1.5). Only Gavroche and Lisa had been wild caught (separately) in Sierra Leone as young animals. All the others were born in captivity and were housed together since birth. Gavroche (with Lisa) had joined the group in 1998. Recordings of cohesion–contact calls could be obtained from six females (Lowina, Maricopa, Chilula, Shawnee, Tilamook, Bela) displaying ‘complete’ structures (see below). The two older females were less ‘vocal’ and produced ‘broken’ calls (see below). The monkeys were housed in an indoor (21 m2×3 m)–outdoor (21 m2×4 m) enclosure. The animals were kept in the indoor part during group observations in order to ensure more easily the identification of partners.

2.2 Data collection and analysis

2.2.1 Social interactions

Focal animal sampling was used, during direct observations using a voice recorder, to collect information on social relationships [36]: one animal was followed for 5 min and every dyadic interaction was recorded. Observations covered a period of 12 continuous days in April 1999 with three sessions a day (at feeding time the morning and the afternoon and at midday). Equal observation time was performed for each focal animal (120 min) and this yielded data on 5159 non-vocal and 249 vocal interactions.

Interactions were divided into the following six categories: pacific interactions (approach, contact, sniffing), social play, avoidance (in response to an approach, avoidance of physical contact), aggression, directed gazes, vocal exchange of cohesion–contact calls (for more details, see Lemasson et al., submitted).

Nonparametric statistics (Chi square or binomial tests – for expected frequencies <5 –, with Bonferroni correction) were used to construct the sociograms (for more details, see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4

Sociograms and vocal sharing. Matriline 1 monkeys are on the left of each sociogram, matriline 2 are on the right. First two letters of names: abbreviations used in sociograms ( Ga ¯vroche, Li ¯sa, Pl ¯ume, Lo ¯wina, Ma ¯ricopa, Ch ¯ilula, Sh ¯awnee, Ti ¯lamook, Be ¯la). The six females whose calls were recorded are in bold type. The arrows are directed from the emitter to the receiver of a behaviour; their thickness is correlated to the frequency of the interaction. Only interactions occurring more often than expected by chance are represented (chi square tests for pacific interactions, directed gazes and binomial tests for social play, vocal exchanges, aggression, avoidance with Bonferroni correction: α=0.05/8). Variants for each female are indicated as exponents.

2.2.2 Sound recordings and analysis

Calls were recorded during 17 30-min sessions in May 1999 with a unidirectional microphone (SENNHEISER MKH815) linked to a stereophonic DAT-recorder (TASCAM DA-P1). Sonograms were computed using an Amiga microcomputer program for sound analysis and synthesis [37]. The calls used for spectrographic analysis were digitised at a 24-kHz sampling rate with an 8-bit sample size. The spectrographic analysis was done using Fast-Fourier Transformations (FFT) with sizes of 256 points for each analysed time window. Resulting spectrograms had a time resolution of 2.49 ms and frequency resolution of 100 Hz.

The cohesion–contact call of Campbell's monkey is composed of first, a low-frequency part and second, a higher pitched whistle, with a large frequency modulation (Fig. 1). These two parts can be produced either separately or associated (C. pogonias [32]; Lemasson et al., in prep.). Moreover, the shapes of the frequency modulation of the second part can be either ‘complete’ or ‘broken’ (Fig. 1) and a further study indicates that the production of these two variants is context-related (Lemasson et al., in preparation). Complete calls were produced approaching a group member and, in adults, only females produced these calls. Therefore, we selected here only the complete calls, which reduced our sample size. A total of 102 calls were selected (mean=17±7 per individual). We focused on the analysis of the second part of the complete call, which was shown to support the highest level of intra- and inter-individual variability while the first part, although present in the calls of all animals, did not show such clear variation patterns (Lemasson and Hausberger, submitted). Given the duration and patterns of variation of these calls (Lemasson and Hausberger, submitted), it appeared that comparisons of sonograms required a more integrative method than simple measurements of frequency or duration parameters, classically used in primates [26,29]. A ‘similarity index’ has been used successfully to distinguish between intra- and inter-population levels in various species of birds and cetaceans [6,35,38] and in particular for European starlings' whistles [34]. Both dolphins' and starlings' whistles show the same type of modulated structure as the calls we were dealing with here. The index software used here was written by Adret-Hausberger [34], derived from Miller [33], and adapted for our species using a custom software (Richard, unpublished). The ‘similarity index’ was calculated by comparing the frequency contours of each pair of sonograms. The program automatically looked for the best superposition along the frequency and duration axis providing a ratio of ‘overlap’ between both contours. Comparisons were made both at the intra- and the inter-individual levels, every call was compared to all other calls (examples are illustrated in Fig. 2a). Other methods, like cross-correlations, were tested but revealed unable to give clear evaluations of similarities/dissimilarities between calls.

Fig. 1

Sonograms of ‘broken’ (left) and ‘complete’ (right) cohesion–contact call.

Fig. 2

Indices and cluster analysis. (a) Examples of pairwise index values resulting from comparisons both at intra- and inter-individual and at intra- and inter-variant levels. (b, c) Cluster analysis based on dendrogram construction classifying the structures emitted by each individual. Two examples of dendrograms: (b) Bela presented three variants (A, B, D); (c) Chilula presented a single variant (B). The dotted line indicate the threshold index=0.31.

We used the UPGMA clustering algorithm to classify the structures produced by each individual (Fig. 2b and c) based on similarity indices (software NTSYS-pc, SAHN clustering program [39]). The use of this method for sound classification has increased over years given its success in songbirds [12,18,38,40,41]. On the basis of the cluster analysis, groups of calls emerged, defining variants (Fig. 2b). A threshold of i=0.31 appeared, which differentiated these variants.

Pairwise indices were then averaged to obtain a mean similarity value for intra- and inter-individual comparisons. Three levels could be discriminated:

  • – intra-individual comparisons revealed that each individual had clearly separated variants (mean index within variants=0.39±0.06, mean index between variants=0.28±0.07, Fig. 3);
  • – variants can be shared by more than one animal as revealed by inter-individual comparisons (mean index within variants=0.34±0.02 [vary from 0.31 to 0.38]);
  • – different variants were clearly divergent (mean index between variants=0.21±0.05; vary from 0.09 to 0.30).

Fig. 3

Distribution of variants in individuals. Horizontal lines: the six variants (A, B, C, D, E, F) presented by the females; columns: the different structures for each female. An example of a call produced by the considered female is given in the cases with the mean index and standard deviation at the intra-individual level. Empty squares show that this variant was not recorded for this animal. For example, variant A is present in the recordings of Bela and Shawnee, but was not recorded for Tilamook, Chilula, Lowina and Maricopa.

Spearman correlations were used to test correlations between intra-variant mean index values and frequencies of occurrence of some category of behaviours.

3 Results

As mentioned in the method part, we could define thresholds that corresponded to different levels of variation. One to three different variants were found per individual, some of them being present in the repertoire of different individuals (Fig. 3). Four variants were shared by at least two individuals: these were variants A (Bela+Shawnee), B (Chilula+Tilamook+Bela+Shawnee), C (Shawnee+Maricopa), D (Bela+Lowina). Only two variants were not shared and they both belonged to Lowina (E, F). All the other individuals shared at least one variant with one other or with several other group members.

Most individuals that shared vocal structures were also preferential social partners (Fig. 4). The two sisters sharing variant A often interacted vocally and pacifically, played together and observed each other frequently. The four females, belonging to two matrilines, sharing variant B, often interacted pacifically (Bela–Shawnee, Tilamook–Shawnee), vocally (Shawnee–Bela–Tilamook), observed one another (Shawnee–Bela–Tilamook) and played together (Tilamook–Chilula–Bela, Shawnee–Bela). The other two convergent pairs (Lowina–Bela, Shawnee–Maricopa) interacted neither pacifically nor agonistically. The more vocally divergent female, Lowina, was totally excluded from the rest of the group during pacific interactions, vocal exchanges and social play (Fig. 4). Furthermore, she aggressed Tilamook and was often aggressed by the adult male. Despite these observations, no significant correlation could be recorded between the index values and the frequency of occurrence of these behaviours except for avoidances. Our six females were not statistically involved in the avoidance network because of the large dominance of the adult female Plume but a significant negative correlation was nevertheless noticed between index values and frequencies of avoidance (Spearman correlation, rs=−0.579, p<0.05).

4 Discussion

The comparison of frequency modulations of cohesion-contact calls using an index of similarity revealed that each individual Campbell's monkey could present one to three different variants. These variants could be shared with one or more group members. This sharing occurred mainly within a social network of play, and included especially one matriline where social interactions were most abundant. One animal (Lowina) that tended to show few interactions and some aggressiveness displayed divergent vocal structures. Moreover, we observed that females that avoided one another less often displayed more similar structures. We were not surprised to discover a correlation with such a behavioural category given the characteristics of social life of arboreal guenons. As closely related species, Campbell's monkeys display a ‘monitor-adjust’ social system with few physical interactions, in particular rare aggressions, but a lot of survey with gaze and avoidance ([42]; Lemasson et al., submitted).

Although these data are still preliminary, given the low number of animals and limited time span of the study, one can wonder how such a sharing could be explained. Two possibilities can be proposed.

  • (1) Call similarity may be due to a convergent morphology and/or way of producing sounds. Hauser thus found similarities in the coo calls of matriline members of rhesus macaques that could be explained by either by genetic transmission of morphology resulting in given ways of opening the mouth or social learning [43]. Such an influence of mouth opening on call structure has been shown in birds [44,45].

    Although we cannot exclude this hypothesis with these preliminary data, especially as matriline members are particularly involved, this would not explain convergence between matrilines. Moreover, as Campbell's monkeys produce their cohesion–contact calls with a closed mouth, other aspects of call production would have to be assessed.

  • (2) Variant sharing may be the result of copying and plasticity in call production. Although social influence on vocal usage has been described in a variety of contexts and species of primates [13–17,46,47], flexibility in vocal structure does not seem to be common. Most observed changes can be attributed to maturation, such as body weight changes and modification of the phonatory organ [48–50]. Recent studies however suggest that some degree of flexibility may exist in primate vocal structures. Some studies of primates present evidence that experience in specific social contexts can modify calls. Female capped gibbons, in the absence of males, produce the male as well as their own contribution to duets [51]. Contact call parameters co-vary with the nature of the contact (tactile, visual, auditory) between pairs in common marmosets [52]. Human caregivers succeeded in conditioning Japanese macaques to modify their vocalizations in a feeding context [28]. Dialects have been described in chimpanzees [25] and Japanese macaques [24]. Nevertheless, despite the fact that primates have got the cognitive capacity to use a very complex vocal communication system, as, after exceptional training, great apes have acquired artificial codes that presented some similarities to human language [53,54], very few studies illustrate vocal convergence in non-human primates. Male chimpanzees match the acoustic characteristics of each other's pant hoots when calling together [26]. Japanese macaques match the frequency parameters of their coo calls to playback signals [27]. The most detailed studies concern captive callitrichids, which change their vocal structures when a new group is introduced in their neighbourhood [29,30,55] and after pairing [31].

As mentioned by Snowdon, the methods used in order to compare calls may be determinant in finding subtle similarities or divergences [30]. In our study, the use of an index of similarity was essential in describing and classifying variants. Such a method is especially appropriate for calls that have a clear frequency modulation beyond a certain duration (e.g., whistles of bottlenose dolphins [56] or European starlings [9]).

Only a long-term study on the group dynamics, observations during ontogeny and comparisons between groups will tell us what are the determinants of these variant sharing (Lemasson et al., in preparation). If vocal copying is involved, although on subtle details of the calls, this may add to the existing evidences of a relation between social affinities and call convergence found at other phylogenetical levels: birds [1,8,12], dolphins [10,11] and humans [57] and finally break a gap in continuity of developmental processes from birds to humans [58].

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Gautier-Hion for useful discussions, J.-P. Richard for having adapted the index software and A. Cloarec for correcting English. This project received financial support from the CNRS program ‘Origine de l'homme, du langage et des langues’.


Bibliographie

[1] E.D. Brown; S.M. Farabaugh What birds with complex social relationships can tell us about vocal learning: vocal sharing in avian groups (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997, pp. 98-127

[2] R.B. Payne; L.L. Payne Field observations, experimental design, and the time and place of learning bird songs (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997, pp. 57-84

[3] B. Griessmann; M. Naguib Song sharing in neighboring and non-neighboring thrush nightingales (Lusciana lusciana) and its implications for communication, Ethology, Volume 108 (2002), pp. 377-387

[4] P.L. Tyack Animal language research needs a broader comparative and evolutionary framework (H.L. Roitblat; L.M. Herman; P. Nachtigall, eds.), Language and Communication: Comparative Perspectives, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1993, pp. 115-152

[5] B. Mc Cowan; D. Reiss Vocal learning in captive bottlenose dolphins: a comparison with humans and nonhuman animals (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1997, pp. 178-207

[6] P.J.O. Miller; D.E. Bain Within-pod variation in the sound production of a pod of killer whales Orcinus orca, Anim. Behav., Volume 60 (2000), pp. 617-628

[7] H. Yurk; L. Barrett-Lennard; J.K.B. Ford; C.O. Matkin Cultural transmission within maternal lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska, Anim Behav., Volume 63 (2002), pp. 1103-1119

[8] M. Hausberger; M.-A. Richard-Yris; L. Henry; L. Lepage; I. Schmidt Song sharing reflects the social organization in a captive group of European starling (Sturnus vulgaris), J. Comp. Psychol., Volume 109 (1995), pp. 222-241

[9] M. Hausberger Social influences on song acquisition and sharing in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1997, pp. 128-156

[10] L.S. Sayigh; P.L. Tyack; R.S. Wells; M.D. Scott Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus: stability and mother-offspring comparisons, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 26 (1990), pp. 247-260

[11] P.L. Tyack; L.S. Sayigh Vocal learning in cetaceans (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1997, pp. 208-233

[12] A.G. Hile; G.F. Striedter Call convergence within groups of female budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus), Ethology, Volume 106 (2000), pp. 1105-1114

[13] R.M. Seyfarth; D.L. Cheney Some general features of vocal development in nonhuman primates (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1997, pp. 249-273

[14] M.D. Hauser Ontogenetic Changes in the Comprehension and Production of Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) vocalizations, J. Comp. Psychol., Volume 103 (1989), pp. 149-158

[15] R.S. Roush; C.T. Snowdon Ontogeny of food-associated calls in cotton-top tamarins, Anim. Behav., Volume 47 (1994), pp. 263-273

[16] R.S. Roush; C.T. Snowdon The effects of social status on food-associated calling behaviour in captive cotton-top tamarins, Anim. Behav., Volume 58 (1999), pp. 1299-1305

[17] B. Mc Cowan; N.V. Franceschini; G.A. Vicino Age differences and developmental trends in alarm peep responses by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus), Am. J. Primatol., Volume 53 (2001), pp. 19-31

[18] L.F. Baptista; S.L.L. Gaunt Social interaction and vocal development in birds (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997, pp. 23-40

[19] C.M. Caldwell; D.K. Caldwell; P.L. Tyack Review of signature-whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (S. Leatherwood; R.R. Reeves, eds.), The Bottlenose Dolphin, Academic Press, New York, 1990, pp. 199-234

[20] V.B. Deecke; J.K.B. Ford; P. Spong Dialect change in resident killer whales: implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission, Anim. Behav., Volume 60 (2000), pp. 629-638

[21] V.M. Janik; J.B. Slater The different roles of social learning in vocal communication, Anim. Behav., Volume 60 (2000), pp. 1-11

[22] P. Winter; P. Handley; D. Ploog; D. Schott Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under natural conditions and under acoustic isolation, Behaviour, Volume 47 (1973), pp. 230-239

[23] J.D. Newman; D. Symmes Inheritance and experience in the acquisition of primate acoustic behaviour (C.T. Snowdon; C.H. Brown; M.R. Petersen, eds.), Primate Communication, Cambridge University Press, New York, 1982, pp. 259-278

[24] S. Green Dialects in Japanese monkeys: vocal learning and cultural transmission of local-specificity behavior?, Z. Tierpsychol., Volume 38 (1975), pp. 304-314

[25] J.-C. Mitani; T. Hasegawa; J. Gros-Louis; P. Marler; R. Byrne Dialects in wild chimpanzees?, Am. J. Primatol., Volume 27 (1992), pp. 233-243

[26] J.-C. Mitani; K.L. Brandt Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees, Ethology, Volume 96 (1994), pp. 233-252

[27] H. Sugiura Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques, Anim. Behav., Volume 55 (1998), pp. 673-687

[28] N. Masataka Attempts by animal caretakers to condition Japanese Macaque vocalization result inadvertently in individual-specific calls (T. Nishida; W.C. Mc Grew; P. Marler, eds.), Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1: Human Origins, University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, 1992, pp. 271-278

[29] A.M. Elowson; C.T. Snowdon Pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pymaea) modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment, Anim. Behav., Volume 47 (1994), pp. 1267-1277

[30] C.T. Snowdon; A.M. Elowson; R.S. Roush Social influences on vocal development in New World primates (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997, pp. 234-248

[31] C.T. Snowdon; A.M. Elowson Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired, Ethology, Volume 105 (1999), pp. 893-908

[32] J.-P. Gautier Interspecific affinities among guenons as deduced from vocalizations (A. Gautier-Hion; F. Bourlière; J.-P. Gautier; J. Kingdon, eds.), A Primate Radiation Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1988, pp. 194-226

[33] E.H. Miller Character and variance shift in acoustic signals of birds (D.E. Kroodsma; E.H. Miller, eds.), Acoustic Communication in Bird, 1, Academic Press, New York, 1982, pp. 253-295

[34] M. Adret-Hausberger Variations dialectales des sifflements de l'étourneau sansonnet sédentaire en Bretagne, Z. Tierpsychol., Volume 62 (1983), pp. 55-71

[35] A.G. Hile; T.K. Plummer; G.F. Striedter Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars Melopsittacus undulatus, Anim. Behav., Volume 59 (2000), pp. 1209-1218

[36] J. Altmann Observational study of behavior: sampling methods, Behaviour, Volume 49 (1974), pp. 227-267

[37] J.-P. Richard Sound analysis and synthesis using an amiga micro-computer, Bioacoustics, Volume 3 (1991), pp. 45-60

[38] S.M. Farabaugh; A. Linzenbold; R.J. Dooling Vocal plasticity in Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): evidence for social factors in the learning of contact calls, J. Comp. Psychol., Volume 108 (1994), pp. 81-92

[39] F.J. Rohlf NTSYS-pc: Numerical Taxonomy and Multivariate Analysis System, version 1.70, Exeter Software, New York, 1992

[40] H. Williams Models for song learning in the zebra finch: fathers or others?, Anim. Behav., Volume 39 (1990), pp. 745-757

[41] C. Cicero; Z.M. Benowitz-Fredericks Song types and variation in insular populations of lincoln's sparrow (Melospiza lincolnii) and comparisons with other Melospiza, The Auk, Volume 117 (2000), pp. 52-64

[42] T.E. Rowell The social system of guenons, compared with baboons, macaques and mangabey (A. Gautier-Hion; F. Bourlière; J.-P. Gautier; J. Kingdon, eds.), A Primate Radiation Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1988, pp. 439-451

[43] M.D. Hauser Articulatory and social factors influence the acoustical structure of Rhesus macaque vocalizations: a learned mode of production?, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Volume 91 (1992), pp. 2175-2179

[44] M. Hausberger; J.M. Black; J.-P. Richard Bill opening and sound spectrum in barnacle goose loud calls: individuals with ‘wide mouths’ have higher pitched voices, Anim. Behav., Volume 42 (1991), pp. 319-322

[45] W. Hoese; J. Podos; N.C. Boetticher; S. Nowicki Vocal tract function in birdsong production: experimental manipulation of beak movements, J. Exp. Biol., Volume 203 (2000), pp. 1845-1855

[46] J.-P. Gautier; A. Gautier Vocal communication within a group of monkeys: an analysis by biotelemetry (C.T. Snowdon; C.H. Brown; M.R. Petersen, eds.), Primate Communication, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1982, pp. 5-39

[47] S. Boinski; C.M. Mitchell Ecological and social factors affecting the vocal behavior of adulte female squirrel monkeys, Ethology, Volume 92 (1992), pp. 316-330

[48] J.-P. Gautier; A. Gautier Communication in old world monkeys (T. Sebeok, ed.), How Animals Communicate, Indiana University Press, Bloomington, 1977, pp. 890-964

[49] M.D. Hauser The evolution of non-human primate vocalizations: effects of phylogeny, body-weight, and social context, Am. Nat., Volume 142 (1993), pp. 528-542

[50] K. Hammerschmidt; J.D. Newmann; M. Champoux; S.J. Suomi Changes in rhesus macaque ‘coo’ vocalizations during early development, Ethology, Volume 106 (2000), pp. 873-886

[51] T. Geissmann Female capped gibbon sings male song, J. Hum. Evol., Volume 12 (1983), pp. 667-671

[52] L. Schrader; D. Todt Contact call parameters covary with social context in common marmosets Callithrix j. jacchus, Anim. Behav., Volume 46 (1993), pp. 1026-1028

[53] M. Tomasello; J. Call; K. Nagell; R. Olguin; M. Carpenter The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study, Primates, Volume 35 (1994), pp. 137-154

[54] E.S. Savage-Rumbaugh; S.L. Williams; T. Furuichi; T. Kano Language perceived: Paniscus branches out (W.C. McGrew; L.F. Marchant; T. Nishida, eds.), Great Ape Asocieties, Cambridge University Press, New York, 1996, pp. 173-184

[55] M. Rukstalis; J.E. Fite; J.A. French Social change affects vocal structure in a Callitrichid primate (Callithrix kuhlii), Ethology, Volume 109 (2003), pp. 327-340

[56] V.M. Janik Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), Science, Volume 289 (2000), pp. 1355-1357

[57] J.L. Locke; C. Snow Social influences on vocal learning in human and nonhuman primates (C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger, eds.), Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997, pp. 274-292

[58] C.T. Snowdon; M. Hausberger Social Influences on Vocal Development, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1997


Commentaires - Politique


Ces articles pourraient vous intéresser

Individual vocal differences of the coo call in Japanese monkeys

Miyuki Ceugniet; Akihiro Izumi

C. R. Biol (2004)


The use of a mirror as a ‘social substitute’ in laboratory birds

Laurence Henry; Katell Le Cars; Maryvonne Mathelier; ...

C. R. Biol (2008)


Song perception in the European starling: hemispheric specialisation and individual variations

Isabelle George; Hugo Cousillas; Jean-Pierre Richard; ...

C. R. Biol (2002)