Plan
Comptes Rendus

Ethology/Éthologie
Begging coordination between siblings in Black-headed Gulls
[Coordination de la quémande entre les jeunes de mouette rieuse]
Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 333 (2010) no. 9, pp. 688-693.

Résumés

Communication behaviours are now considered from a signallers–receivers network perspective. This concept seems well suited to the study of interactions between parents and offspring in birds, so far mainly treated as a dyadic signalling system involving the brood or a single chick as a signaller and the parent as a receiver. Family conflicts over resource allocation drive parent–offspring and sib–sib communication. In the Black-headed Gull Larus ridibundus, parents respond to the whole-brood begging intensity and siblings often synchronize their begging signalling thus limiting individual effort. By monitoring five nests of two-chick broods during the whole rearing period in the nest, we show how an intra-brood simultaneity of begging emerges from successive phases of solitary begging of junior and senior nestlings. Although this result remains preliminary due to the sample size, it underlines a dynamical aspect of chicks’ behaviour. Because they always favour coordinated begging and because they elevate their response threshold across the rearing period, parents may play a major role in the plasticity of begging behaviour.

Les comportements de communication sont maintenant considérés sous l’angle des réseaux d’émetteurs–récepteurs. Ce concept semble bien adapté aux interactions parents–jeunes chez les oiseaux, qui ont pourtant été principalement étudiées comme systèmes dyadiques impliquant la portée où un unique poussin comme émetteur et le parent comme récepteur. Les conflits familiaux concernant l’allocation des ressources dirigent la communication parents–poussins ainsi que les interactions entre poussins. Chez la mouette rieuse Larus ridibundus, les parents répondent à l’intensité de la quémande globale de la portée et les poussins synchronisent souvent leur quémande, limitant ainsi l’effort individuel. En observant des portées de deux poussins pendant toute la période d’élevage de cinq nids différents, nous montrons comment cette simultanéité intra-portée émerge depuis des phases de quémande solitaire des poussins juniors et seniors. Bien que ce résultat soit encore préliminaire suite à la petite taille de l’échantillon, il souligne un aspect dynamique du comportement des poussins. Parce qu’ils favorisent toujours la quémande simultanée et parce qu’ils élèvent leur seuil de réponse au cours de la période d’élevage, les parents sont susceptibles de jouer un rôle majeur dans la plasticité du comportement de quémande.

Métadonnées
Reçu le :
Accepté le :
Publié le :
DOI : 10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002
Keywords: Animal communication, Begging behaviour, Parent–offspring conflict, Black-headed gull
Mot clés : Communication animale, Comportement de quémande, Conflit parents–jeunes, Mouette rieuse
Alain Blanc 1, 2 ; Nicolas Ogier 1, 2 ; Angélique Roux 1, 2 ; Sébastien Denizeau 1, 2 ; Nicolas Mathevon 1, 2

1 Équipe de neuro-éthologie sensorielle ENES/CNPS, CNRS UMR 8195, université de Saint-Étienne, 23, rue Michelon, 42023 Saint-Étienne, France
2 Centre national de la recherche scientifique, centre de neurosciences Paris-Sud, UMR 8195, France
@article{CRBIOL_2010__333_9_688_0,
     author = {Alain Blanc and Nicolas Ogier and Ang\'elique Roux and S\'ebastien Denizeau and Nicolas Mathevon},
     title = {Begging coordination between siblings in {Black-headed} {Gulls}},
     journal = {Comptes Rendus. Biologies},
     pages = {688--693},
     publisher = {Elsevier},
     volume = {333},
     number = {9},
     year = {2010},
     doi = {10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002},
     language = {en},
}
TY  - JOUR
AU  - Alain Blanc
AU  - Nicolas Ogier
AU  - Angélique Roux
AU  - Sébastien Denizeau
AU  - Nicolas Mathevon
TI  - Begging coordination between siblings in Black-headed Gulls
JO  - Comptes Rendus. Biologies
PY  - 2010
SP  - 688
EP  - 693
VL  - 333
IS  - 9
PB  - Elsevier
DO  - 10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002
LA  - en
ID  - CRBIOL_2010__333_9_688_0
ER  - 
%0 Journal Article
%A Alain Blanc
%A Nicolas Ogier
%A Angélique Roux
%A Sébastien Denizeau
%A Nicolas Mathevon
%T Begging coordination between siblings in Black-headed Gulls
%J Comptes Rendus. Biologies
%D 2010
%P 688-693
%V 333
%N 9
%I Elsevier
%R 10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002
%G en
%F CRBIOL_2010__333_9_688_0
Alain Blanc; Nicolas Ogier; Angélique Roux; Sébastien Denizeau; Nicolas Mathevon. Begging coordination between siblings in Black-headed Gulls. Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 333 (2010) no. 9, pp. 688-693. doi : 10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2010.06.002/

Version originale du texte intégral

1 Introduction

Since the recent focus on animal communication networks, empirical and experimental as well as mathematical approaches have shown that individuals interacting with conspecifics use elaborate signalling strategies [1–3]. However, the emergence of these behaviours remains poorly understood. The study of parent–offspring relationships in birds may offer an opportunity to fill this gap [4]. In most bird species, offspring solicit parental feeding by performing demonstrative acoustic and visual signalling (begging behaviour, [5]). Within broods of several chicks, between-sibling competition in addition to the well-known “parent–offspring” conflict can both drive the dynamics of the family communication network [6–9]. Other factors such as the presence of predators may complicate the pattern [10–12]. With a time scale spreading from hatching to juveniles’ fledging, the parent–offspring communication network represents an interesting paradigm to decipher the developmental timing of signalling strategies in an interactive context.

In birds, begging behaviours have been the subject of numerous empirical and theoretical investigations [8]. Parent–offspring conflict suggests that chicks’ solicitation may be greater than is optimal for the parents [6]. However, this conflict can show a stable resolution: begging intensity and frequency may represent an honest cue of the need of young with parents responding in proportion [5]. Besides being possibly in conflict with parental interests, a chick may compete for food with its siblings. Parental food supply may be limited – from the chicks’ point of view at least –, a situation that can increase competition within the brood [7]. In some species, these conflicts may result in the death of some of the chicks [8]. Visible expressions of intra-brood competition can be fight for getting the closest position to the feeding parent, exaggeration of begging signals and physical aggression between siblings [8]. In this context, parents and offspring constitute a complex communication network with multiple signallers interacting together [4]. In spite of the efforts that have been performed to decipher the various strategies used by the network protagonists, the developmental aspects have been almost neglected up to now. However, it should be of primary importance, given the considerable changes that affect chicks’ biology (e.g., in terms of morphology, alimentary need, behavioural repertoire and many other aspects) during their breeding period. It is then poorly known how and in what extent behavioural strategies change from hatching to fledging. These changes are of course driven by the dramatic maturation events that affect growing chicks. These events are the result of both internal (genetic) and external (e.g., parental response to chick's signals) factors. In the present study, we propose to describe this kind of behavioural change in the chicks of the Black-headed gull Larus ridibundus.

In the Black-headed Gull, parents respond to chick solicitation by regurgitating food onto the ground. This specificity means that, conversely to situation occurring in many birds, Black-headed Gull parents do not allocate food to each chick individually. By monitoring both chick behaviour and parental response, a recent study [13,15] has shown that parents adjust their response – in terms of number of regurgitations – to the nest begging intensity. That is, parents are sensitive to the overall begging signal that emerges from the nest whether it comes from one isolated chick or from several chicks begging together. Moreover, sibling chicks apparently take into account this nest-adjusted parental response by synchronizing their begging signals, thus limiting individual effort. The study [13,15] focused on 8- to 15-days old chicks, and thus the ontogeny of this process – how the synchronization between chicks arises – is unknown. The present article investigates this question by monitoring broods of Black-headed Gulls from hatching to nest emancipation.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study area and study species

The fieldwork was carried out at La Ronze pond (Forez plain 4° 13′ 33″ East longitude and 45° 34′ 31″ North latitude, France), a 12 ha pond which supports a large colony of Black-headed Gulls (4000 breeding pairs). Clutches of this species typically contain three eggs [14,16]. However, as nests with two chicks are the most typical in our study area, we chose to focus on two-chick broods for the present study. The Black-headed Gull is a semi-altricial species: chicks remain in the nest until the age of 3 weeks and both parents participate in chick rearing. Three age stages (each of about one week long) are commonly used to describe the nestling period: downy chicks (stage 1), chicks with first white feathers on the abdomen (stage 2), chicks with feathered wings and first black stains on the head (stage 3) [15,17].

2.2 Behavioural observations

Each nest studied (n = 5) was marked individually with a numbered plastic stake and monitored from egg laying to chick emancipation. Chicks (n = 10, two birds per nest) were individually identified by two bands (a metallic band numbered according the standard of the Museum national d’histoire naturelle and a coloured band to allow visual identification at a distance). Observations were conducted from a floating hiding place, remaining at 3–5 meters from the nest. Broods were observed during one hour twice a day over a 3-weeks breeding period, for a total of more than 650 begging sessions. Daily observations were organized in two periods (morning: 7–14 h and afternoon: 14–18 h), with a balanced pattern between nests to avoid pseudo-replication, meaning that the order of observations of the five nests permuted from day to day.

Gull begging displays involve visual and acoustical components [16,18]. The following 1–4 level ethological scale adapted from [13,15] was used to qualify the intensity of a begging bout of an individual. Whenever a chick begged, we get the score of its begging behaviour by noting the number of hunched postures (HP) and of beak stimulations (BS), and the rate (low LO = calls emitted during less than 30% of the begging bout duration, medium ME = calls emitted during 30 to 60% of the begging bout, or high HI = calls emitted during more than 60% of the begging bout) of calls. The 1–4 scores were defined as follows:

1 = (no HP + 1 BS) or (1 HP + no BS) + LO.

2 = (1 HP + 1 BS) or (no HP + 2 BS) or (2 HP + no BS) + LO or ME.

3 = (1 HP + 2 BS) or (2 HP + 1 BS) or (2 HP + 2 BS) + ME.

4 = (1 to more than 2 HP + 3 or more BS) or (3 or more HP + 1 to more than 2 BS) + HI.

When two chicks begged together, the total begging intensity emerging from the nest was given by the sum of both individual begging intensities obtained with the ethological scale.

As mentioned in the introduction, Black-headed Gull chicks may beg alone or together. During observations, three different kinds of begging bouts were thus noted: solitary begging of either a junior or senior chick and synchronized begging of both chicks. Begging efficiency was assessed by monitoring parental response (food regurgitation or not).

2.3 Statistical analysis

The respective numbers of begging situations (solitary or synchronized) were summed per brood and per day according to the nestling stage and compared using Wilcoxon matched pair signed ranks test. The same test allowed the comparison of chick begging intensities during synchronized begging sessions. Efficiency in eliciting parental regurgitation and begging intensity distribution were respectively compared between the various types of begging bouts using chi-square tests. Finally, we used a Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA by Ranks test to compare begging intensity distributions at different stages.

3 Results

3.1 Frequency and intensity of individual and synchronized begging sequences

Fig. 1 reports the relative frequency of individual and synchronized begging sequences as a function of nestling stage. Although chicks preferentially begged alone at stage 1 and 2, synchronized begging emerged at this age (proportion of synchronized begging is significantly less than junior and senior chicks solitary begging, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test, P < 0.05 for each comparison, see Fig. 1 for quantitative values). However, the frequency of synchronized begging events strongly increased during stage 3. At this age, a chick – either the junior or the senior one – shared about 50% of its begging sessions with its sibling (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1

Frequency distribution of solitary (junior and senior chicks) and coordinated begging events over the three stages of the nesting period (n = number of observed begging events). Whereas solitary begging predominates during the two first developmental stages, the relative importance of simultaneous begging increases during the third stage.

When begging alone at stage 1, the junior chick demonstrated a higher begging intensity than the senior chick (chi-square test, P = 0.029). Moreover, this difference of investment between chicks was perceptible, but not significant, when comparing intensities during synchronized begging sessions (Fig. 2A, Wilcoxon test, P = 0.09). Conversely, at stage 2, the senior chick increased overall begging intensity to the level of the junior chick (chi-square test, P = 0.31 for solitary begging; Wilcoxon test, P = 0.85 for synchronized begging, Fig. 2B). However, stage 2 begging sessions were mostly asymmetrical, combining a high begging intensity of one chick with low intensity from the other. Thus alternating behaviour occurred from each sibling during stage 2 (Fig. 2B). At stage 3, junior and senior chicks still showed different begging intensities. However, the pattern was modified when junior and senior chicks often begged together at a high level (Fig. 2C). As a consequence, the total level of begging intensity emerging from the nest raised considerably at this age (medians = 3, 4 and 6 respectively at stage 1, 2 and 3; comparing stage 1 vs. stage 2: P = 0.11, comparing stage 2 vs. stage 3: P = 0.005, Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA by Ranks test).

Fig. 2

Distributions of begging intensities for senior and junior chicks during coordinated begging events over the three stages of the nesting period. The apparition of coordinated sessions of high intensity is to be noted at stage 3.

3.2 Parental response

Except for the stage 1 junior chick, synchronized begging events are by far more efficient than solitary events in eliciting parental response (chi-square test, P < 0.01 at stage 1, and P < 0.001 at stages 2 and 3; Fig. 3). Indeed, synchronized events elicit parental regurgitation in more than 50% of cases whereas solitary begging hardly reaches 20% success. The efficiency of the stage 1 junior chick when begging alone (about 30% success) is likely linked to its high-intensity solitary begging.

Fig. 3

Parental response to solitary and coordinated begging over the 3 stages of the nesting period. Coordinated begging is always more efficient in eliciting parental regurgitation.

As illustrated by Fig. 4, parental response threshold rises with chick age. The older the chicks, the higher the parental threshold is, meaning that parents become more reluctant to respond to begging events of low total intensity as chicks grow up (stage 3 different form stage 2, P = 0.01, Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA by Ranks test).

Fig. 4

Frequency distributions of successful begging sessions over the 3 stages of the nesting period. Arrows indicate median values of distributions.

4 Discussion

The present longitudinal study shows significant changes in gull chick begging behaviour and parental responses over the nestling period. Although the small sample size incite to caution (5 nests monitored – but more than 650 begging events observed), the main finding is the development of coordinated begging bouts of high intensity. High-intensity begging behaviours appeared as soon as the end of the first week in the nest, but their coordination between the two chicks emerged during the last week of the nestling period. The most parsimonious explanation for a high coordination between chicks could be the increasing of the frequency of begging behaviours with age: if chicks beg more often, they would increase their chance to beg together. However, as there is usually 3–5 begging events/hour, each lasting around 1 minute, high proportions of synchronized begging are unlikely to be explained by chance only (for further considerations on this aspect, see [13,15]).

Previous observations on the same species have revealed another aspect of begging coordination when comparing nests with one, two or three nestlings; the more siblings there are, the more they coordinate their begging while decreasing the number of individual begging bouts [13,15]. In the Black-headed Gull, begging behaviour thus becomes a collective enterprise as described with banded mongoose Mungo mungo in a slightly different context [17,19]. The age-linked increase of this behavioural coordination may be related to modifications of the parental response threshold to chick begging. When the brood is less than one week old, parental regurgitations occur after weak begging sessions, whereas only intense begging is able to elicit parental response at stage 3. It is striking that this rise in parental threshold co-occurs with the qualitative coordination of begging behaviour. Such coordination increases the whole-brood begging intensity, and it is known that the rate of parental food delivery to nestling broods by parents is positively related to the total begging intensity by the brood ([13]; the present study; for examples in other species, see e.g. [18–20]).

It is generally thought that resolution of intrafamilial conflict depends critically on two mechanisms: (i) how changes in parental investment supply affect begging demand; and (ii) how changes in demand affect parental investment supply [21]. In this way, our observations reveal a possible adaptive response of offspring begging strategy to parental response. Whether begging by bird nestlings is primarily a solicitation of parental care based on degree of need or competition with siblings to exploit parental response to the loudest caller depends on whether and how parents control the allocation of provisioning [22]. A characteristic feature of the Black-headed Gull feeding behaviour is that parents regurgitate food on the nest ground and they do not, like passerine species for instance, allocate food individually to each chick. Although in the absence of individual food allocation, parents may be considered as passive providers, they could exert some influence on offspring behaviour when significantly favouring coordinated versus individual begging. Our results show that this trend is present from the beginning and across the whole rearing period spent in the nest, suggesting an effect of parental choice being imposed on the nestlings. This constraint on chicks may be more pronounced as chicks grow up since parental response threshold to begging intensity increases with time. In response, chicks adjust their begging behaviour by way of greater coordination. Likely driven by parental pressure, this adjustment should involve learning processes [23–25].

Our study provides the opportunity to compare the evolution of begging behaviour between junior and senior chicks. Overall, both chicks follow the same pattern, preferring synchronous begging especially at stage 3. However, there behaviour differs at stage 1, where junior chick begs more often and more intensively than its senior sibling. This result is in line with other works that revealed that the larger and competitively superior nestling often begs less than its nest mates [23,24,26–29]. A previous study [23] suggested that from the start, late-hatched nestlings experience severe competition and so learn that parental rewards are gained only after intense solicitation. In the Black-headed Gull, this divergence in begging patterns between senior and junior nestlings disappeared during the second stage of chick development, due to an increase in begging effort by senior chicks. To our best knowledge, the sole example of the same process of begging re-equilibration has been described in crimson rosella Platycercus elegans [30].

In line with recent works [31–34], the present study emphasizes that the dynamics of begging behaviour is a complex phenomenon, likely to be largely influenced by parental behaviour. Through favouring synchronized begging across the whole rearing period in the nest, Black-headed Gull parents may play a major role in the emergence of this behaviour among chicks. This supports the idea that nestlings can follow sophisticated conditional strategies and that their begging behaviour, far from being innate and stereotyped, may show large amount of learning plasticity.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors state that they have no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

We thank J.D. Lebreton and P.A. Crochet for their help in the field, and M. Dorkenoo for statistical advice. This work was supported by the université de Saint-Étienne and Saint-Étienne Métropole. NM is funded by the Institut universitaire de France.


Bibliographie

[1] C. Vignal; N. Mathevon; S. Mottin Audience drives male songbird response to partner's voice, Nature, Volume 430 (2004), pp. 448-451

[2] P.K. McGregor Animal Communication Networks, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2005

[3] N. Mathevon; T. Aubin; J. Vielliard; M.L. da Silva; F. Sebe; D. Boscolo Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information, PLoS ONE, Volume 3 (2008) no. 2

[4] A.G. Horn; M.L. Leonard Nestling begging as a communication network (McGregor, ed.), Animal Communication Networks, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2005

[5] R. Kilner; R.A. Johnstone Begging the question: are offspring solicitation behaviours signals of need?, Trends Ecol. Evol., Volume 12 (1997), pp. 11-15

[6] R.L. Trivers Parent–offspring conflict, Am. Zool., Volume 14 (1974), pp. 249-264

[7] M.L. Leonard; A.G. Horn Need and nestmates affect begging in tree swallows, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 42 (1998), pp. 431-436

[8] A.E. Budden; J. Wright Begging in nestling birds (V. Nolan; C.F. Thompson, eds.), Current Ornithology, 16, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York, 2001, pp. 83-118

[9] M.L. Leonard; A.G. Horn Begging in the absence of parents by nestling tree swallows, Behav. Ecol., Volume 12 (2001), pp. 501-505

[10] J.V. Briskie; P.R. Martin; T.E. Martin Nest predation and the evolution of nestling begging calls, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B., Volume 266 (1999), pp. 2153-12153

[11] D.G. Haskell The effect of predation on begging-call evolution in nestling wood warblers, Anim. Behav., Volume 57 (1999), pp. 893-901

[12] R.D. Magrath; B.J. Pitcher; A.H. Dalziell How to be fed but not eaten: nestling responses to parental food calls and the sound of a predator's footsteps, Anim. Behav., Volume 74 (2007), pp. 1117-1129

[13] N. Mathevon; I. Charrier Parent–offspring conflict and the coordination of siblings in gulls, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B (Suppl.), Volume 271 (2004), p. S145-S147

[14] S. Cramp; K.E.L. Simmons Handbook of the birds of Europe, the Middle East and North Africa, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 1983

[15] J.D. Lebreton; P. Isenmann Dynamique de la population camarguaise de mouettes rieuses Larus ridibundus L.: un modèle mathématique, Terre et Vie, Volume 30 (1976), pp. 529-549

[16] J. Fjeldsa The Black-headed gull, Av-media, Copenhagen, 1978

[17] M.B.V. Bell Cooperative begging in banded mongoose pups, Curr. Biol., Volume 17 (2007), pp. 717-721

[18] H. Bengtsson; O. Ryden Parental feeding rate in relation to begging behavior in asynchronously hatched broods of the great tit Parus major, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 12 (1983), pp. 243-251

[19] U. Ottosson; J. Bäckman; H.G. Smith Begging affects parental effort in the pied flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 41 (1997), pp. 381-384

[20] J. Wright Helpers-at-the-nest have the same provisioning rule as parents: experimental evidence from play-backs of chick begging, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 42 (1998), pp. 423-429

[21] D.W. Mock; G.A. Parker The evolution of sibling rivalry, Oxford University Press, New York, 1998

[22] G.A. Parker Intrafamilial conflict and parental investment: a synthesis, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, Volume 357 (2002), pp. 295-307

[23] J. Stamps; A. Clark; P. Arrowood; B. Kus Begging behavior in budgerigars, Ethology, Volume 81 (1989), pp. 177-192

[24] P.A. Cotton; J. Wright; A. Kacelnik Chick begging strategies in relation to brood hierarchies and hatching asynchrony, Amer. Nat., Volume 153 (1999), pp. 412-420

[25] H. Kedar; M.A. Rodriguez-Girones; S. Yedvab; D.W. Winkler; A. Lotem Experimental evidence for offspring learning in parent-offspring communication, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, Volume 267 (2000), pp. 1723-1727

[26] R. Kilner, When do canary parents respond to nestling signals of need? Proc. R. Soc. K. Price, R. Ydenberg, Begging and provisioning in broods of asynchronously hatched yellow-headed blackbird nestlings, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 37 (1995) 201–208.

[27] K. Price; H. Harvey; R. Ydenberg Begging tactics of nestling yellow-headed blackbirds, Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus, in relation to need, Anim. Behav., Volume 51 (1996), pp. 421-435

[28] A. Lotem Higher levels of begging behavior by small nestlings: a case of a negatively correlated handicap, Israel J. Zool., Volume 44 (1998), pp. 29-45

[29] E.A. Krebs Begging and food distribution in crimson rosella (Platycercus elegans) broods: why don’t hungry chicks beg more?, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 50 (2001), pp. 20-30

[30] E. Bulmer; P. Celis; D. Gil Parent-absent begging: evidence for sibling honesty and cooperation in the spotless starling (Sturnus unicolor), Behav. Ecol., Volume 19 (2008), pp. 279-284

[31] N.J. Raihani; A.R. Ridley Adult vocalizations during provisioning: offspring response and postfledging benefits in wild pied babblers, Anim. Behav., Volume 74 (2007), pp. 1303-1309

[32] A. Roulin; L.F. Bersier Nestling barn owls beg more intensely in the presence of their mother than in the presence of their father, Anim. Behav., Volume 74 (2007), pp. 1099-1106

[33] C. Valderrabano-Ibarra; I. Brumon; H. Drummond Development of a linear dominance hierarchy in nestling birds, Anim. Behav., Volume 74 (2007), pp. 1705-1714

[34] M. Tanner; M. Kolliker; H. Richner Differential food allocation by male and female great tit, Parus major, parents: are parents or offspring in control?, Anim. Behav., Volume 75 (2008), pp. 1563-1569


Commentaires - Politique


Ces articles pourraient vous intéresser

Response to begging calls by Zebra Finch parents: “First come, first served” rule may overcome a parental preference between chicks

Florence Levréro; Alain Blanc; Nicolas Mathevon

C. R. Biol (2012)


Begging calls support offspring individual identity and recognition by zebra finch parents

Florence Levréro; Laureline Durand; Clémentine Vignal; ...

C. R. Biol (2009)


Potential for individual recognition in acoustic signals: a comparative study of two gulls with different nesting patterns

Nicolas Mathevon; Isabelle Charrier; Pierre Jouventin

C. R. Biol (2003)