Outline
Comptes Rendus

Neurosciences/Neurosciences
Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space
Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 342 (2019) no. 3-4, pp. 97-100.

Abstract

Under visual guidance, healthy subjects usually misbisect radial lines farther than, and vertical lines above the true center. It was suggested that radial and vertical misbisection depended on the presence of an attentional bias toward far/upper space. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether such attentional bias depends on a single mechanism or on separate mechanisms. Ninety participants were asked to bisect lines radially and vertically oriented. The results confirmed the presence of a consistent bisection bias farther than (radial lines), and above (vertical lines) the true center. Furthermore, there was a significant correlation between radial and vertical bisection errors. These findings suggest that a single neural mechanism is involved in producing the attentional bias toward far/upper space.

Metadata
Received:
Accepted:
Published online:
DOI: 10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003
Keywords: Line bisection, Radial lines, Vertical lines, Attention bias, Bisection bias

Sergio Chieffi 1; Clara Castaldi 1; Girolamo Di Maio 1; Marco La Marra 1; Antonietta Messina 1; Vincenzo Monda 1; Ines Villano 1

1 Dipartimento di Medicina Sperimentale, Università degli Studi della Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 80128 Napoli, Italy
@article{CRBIOL_2019__342_3-4_97_0,
     author = {Sergio Chieffi and Clara Castaldi and Girolamo Di Maio and Marco La Marra and Antonietta Messina and Vincenzo Monda and Ines Villano},
     title = {Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space},
     journal = {Comptes Rendus. Biologies},
     pages = {97--100},
     publisher = {Elsevier},
     volume = {342},
     number = {3-4},
     year = {2019},
     doi = {10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003},
     language = {en},
}
TY  - JOUR
AU  - Sergio Chieffi
AU  - Clara Castaldi
AU  - Girolamo Di Maio
AU  - Marco La Marra
AU  - Antonietta Messina
AU  - Vincenzo Monda
AU  - Ines Villano
TI  - Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space
JO  - Comptes Rendus. Biologies
PY  - 2019
SP  - 97
EP  - 100
VL  - 342
IS  - 3-4
PB  - Elsevier
DO  - 10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003
LA  - en
ID  - CRBIOL_2019__342_3-4_97_0
ER  - 
%0 Journal Article
%A Sergio Chieffi
%A Clara Castaldi
%A Girolamo Di Maio
%A Marco La Marra
%A Antonietta Messina
%A Vincenzo Monda
%A Ines Villano
%T Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space
%J Comptes Rendus. Biologies
%D 2019
%P 97-100
%V 342
%N 3-4
%I Elsevier
%R 10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003
%G en
%F CRBIOL_2019__342_3-4_97_0
Sergio Chieffi; Clara Castaldi; Girolamo Di Maio; Marco La Marra; Antonietta Messina; Vincenzo Monda; Ines Villano. Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space. Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 342 (2019) no. 3-4, pp. 97-100. doi : 10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2019.03.003/

Version originale du texte intégral

1 Introduction

Line bisection is a perceptual-motor task commonly used in neurological examinations for assessing hemispatial neglect. The task requires to localize and mark with a pencil the center of a line drawn on a sheet of paper. When patients with hemispatial neglect bisect horizontal lines, they place the subjective midpoint toward the ipsilesional side [1]. Neglect may occur along horizontal, radial, and vertical dimensions of space [2–4].

Bisection performance has been extensively studied also in healthy individuals. Several factors may influence their performance, such as spatial orientation [3,4], learned reading direction [5], presence of contextual stimuli [6–8], and the age of the subjects [9]. Some researchers found a tendency of healthy subjects to bisect horizontal lines to the left of the true midpoint [4], others reported considerable interindividual variability in the direction and extent of errors [10,11]. Conversely, a consistent bisection bias was reported for bisection of radial and vertical lines [3,12,13]. Healthy individuals tend to bisect radial lines farther than, and vertical lines above the true midpoint [3,12,13]. Shelton et al. [3] attributed this bias to perceptual/attentional factors. During visual exploration, attention is preferentially distributed away from the body (“far peripersonal space”), since the visual system is tuned to detect distant stimuli [3].

Experimental evidence suggests the existence of separate neural systems involved in shifting attention along radial and vertical space dimensions. Occipitoparietal areas would shift attention toward the near/lower space [2,14–17], occipitotemporal areas toward the far/upper space [3,14,18]. An important question is whether the shifting of attention along the radial and vertical dimensions of space depends on a single mechanism or on separate mechanisms. We tried to answer this question by asking a group of healthy participants to bisect radial and vertical lines. Our prediction was as follows:

  • • the presence of a significant correlation between radial and vertical bisection errors would have supported the hypothesis that a single system was involved in producing the attentional bias directed toward far/upper space;
  • • the absence of a significant correlation between radial and vertical bisection errors would have supported the hypothesis that separate systems were involved in producing the attentional bias directed toward far/upper space.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Ninety healthy, right-handed subjects (70 women and 20 men) participated in the study. Their mean age was 21.8 years (SD 2.5, range 19–28). The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [19] was used to measure handedness (mean score = 95.6, SD = 6.2, range = 85–100). The participants were students at the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”. They reported having normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The experiment was approved by the ethics committee and was performed in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Participants gave written informed consent to take part in the study.

2.2 Stimuli

The stimuli were black lines 24 cm long and 1.0 mm wide. They were drawn and centered on a sheet of white paper 29.7 cm × 21.0 cm.

2.3 Procedure

The participants sat in a comfortable chair in front of a table where the stimuli were presented along the radial and vertical axes. Radial stimuli were presented on the table top, 45 cm below eye level, at the intersection of the transverse and midsagittal planes. Their midpoint was 35 cm from the participant's body. Vertical lines were presented on a wall, 35 cm front the participant, at the intersection of the frontal and midsagittal plane, and their midpoint was at the subject's eye level.

The experimenter presented the stimuli one at a time. The participants were asked to bisect the lines using a pencil held with their right hand. Each participant bisected a total of 20 lines, administered in two blocks of trials [(radial vs. vertical axis) × ten presentations]. In each block, the lines were presented in either the radial or the vertical axis, and the order of the two blocks was counterbalanced across subjects. Line bisection error (LBE) corresponded to the distance of the subjective midpoint from the true center. It was measured to an accuracy of 0.5 mm. Deviations farther than (radial lines), or above (vertical lines) the true center were assigned a positive value, whereas errors nearer than, or below the true center were given negative values. The standard deviation of the LBE, calculated for each condition, was the variable bisection error (VBE). Variability scores quantify the scatter of subjective midpoints and are sensitive to variability or inconsistency in responding.

2.4 Statistical analysis

The mean values of LBE and VBE were analyzed. They were subjected to one-way analyses of variance with axis (radial vs. vertical) as the within-subjects factor. Furthermore, to investigate the direction of misbisection in each axis condition, one-sample, two-tailed t tests (df = 89) were also performed comparing LBE with the null set (true center). Significance level was fixed at p = 0.025, considering an overall 0.05 level divided by the number of comparisons, according to Bonferroni procedure. Finally, to investigate whether there was a relationship between radial and vertical LBEs, we calculated the Pearson correlation coefficient between the two variables.

3 Results

The mean values of LBE and VBE are graphically reported in Fig. 1a and b, respectively. The axis significantly influenced LBE (F(1,89) = 43.65, p < 0.0001; radial axis = 3.89 mm; vertical axis = 7.49 mm). One-sample, two-tailed t tests (df = 89) showed that subjects bisected radial lines farther than, and vertical lines above the true midpoint (radial axis: t = 8.09; p < 0.0001; vertical axis: t = 13.39; P < 0.0001). No significant axis effect was found on VBE (F(1,89) = 3.42, p = 0.07; radial axis = 4.12 mm; vertical axis = 3.77 mm).

Fig. 1

Mean values of line bisection errors (a) and variable bisection errors (b) in radial and vertical axis conditions. Positive values of line bisection errors indicate that misbisection was farther than (radial condition) or above (vertical condition) the true center. Error bars indicate SE.

Finally, a significant positive correlation between radial and vertical errors was observed (r = 0.46, t = 4.86, df = 88, p < 0.0001).

4 Discussion

The main findings of the present study were:

  • • participants bisected radial lines farther than, and vertical lines above the true centre. This result is in line with previous studies [3,12,13]. However, the bisection bias in the vertical condition was greater than that in the radial condition. Interestingly, VBE did not differ between the two spatial conditions. Variable scores quantify the scatter of subjective midpoints and are sensitive to variability or inconsistency in responding. Therefore, in our experiment, the consistency of bisection performance was similar in the radial and vertical conditions;
  • • a significant positive correlation between radial and vertical LBEs was present. This observation support the hypothesis that a single neural mechanism was involved in producing attentional bias directed toward far/upper space.

As mentioned above, experimental evidence suggests the existence of separate attentional systems involved in shifting attention toward far/upper vs. near/lower space [14]. The occipitoparietal (dorsal) system would shift attention toward near/lower space, occipitotemporal (ventral) system toward far/upper space [2,3,15–18]. Drain and Reuter-Lorenz [14] suggested that the two systems are in mutually inhibitory control of attention orienting. Damage to occipitoparietal regions would lead to a concomitant disinhibition in occipitotemporal activity and a far/upward orienting bias. Conversely, occipitotemporal damage would produce a disinhibition in occipitoparietal activity and a near/downward orienting bias [14]. Then, it is possible that the bisection biases observed in healthy individuals might depend on an imbalance between neural attentional mechanisms. In other words, occipitotemporal activity that shifts attention toward far/upper space would prevail over occipitoparietal activity that shifts attention toward near/lower space. Note that when the participants foveated the central region of the line to localize the subjective midpoint [20], the images of the distal (radial lines) and upper (vertical lines) portions of the line was projected onto the inferior retina (and processed primarily by the occipitotemporal stream), and the images of the proximal (radial lines) and lower (vertical lines) portions onto the superior retina (and processed primarily by the occipitoparietal stream). This might have magnified the magnitude of the distal and upper portion of the line. Previous studies showed that the magnitude of attended stimuli appears magnified compared to that of unattended stimuli [21–24]. In this way, the magnification of the distal (radial lines) and upper (vertical lines) portion of the line might have moved forward and upward the location of the subjective midpoint, respectively.

An important issue that has been addressed in the present study was whether the attentional bias toward far/upper space depended on a single mechanism or on separate mechanisms. To answer this question, we examined whether there was a relationship between radial and vertical bisection errors. Our results showed that a significant correlation between the errors in the two spatial dimensions was present. This observation supports the hypothesis that a single mechanism, depending on the activity of occipitotemporal stream, is involved in shifting attention toward far/upper space.

In favor of this hypothesis, there are some neurological observations. Shelton et al. [3] reported a patient with far radial and upper vertical neglect. The patient presented ischemic lesions involving both occipital and inferior temporal lobes.

Shelton et al. [3] asked the patient to bisect visually presented lines and wooden rods while blindfolded. They observed that the patient bisected radial lines (and rods) nearer than, and vertical lines (and rods) below the subjective midpoint of the control group. A completely opposite behavior was observed by Mennemeier et al. [16] in a patient with bilateral posterior parietal lobe, and near/lower neglect. The patient localized the midpoint of visually presented lines and wooden dowels, while blindfolded, farther than and above the subjective midpoint of the control group [16].

In conclusion, both our observations on healthy individuals and previous observations on brain-damaged patients [3,16] suggest that a single neural mechanism is involved in shifting attention toward far/upper space.


References

[1] E. Bisiach; G. Vallar Hemineglect in humans (F. Boller; J. Grafman, eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology, 1, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1988, pp. 195-222

[2] Z.S. Rapcsak; C.R. Cimino; K.M. Heilman Altitudinal neglect, Neurology, Volume 38 (1988), pp. 277-281 | DOI

[3] A.P. Shelton; D. Bowers; K.M. Heilman Peripersonal and vertical neglect, Brain, Volume 113 (1990), pp. 191-205 | DOI

[4] D.K. Bowers; M. Heilman Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task, Neuropsychologia, Volume 18 (1980), pp. 491-498

[5] S. Chokron; M. Imbert Influence of reading habits on line bisection, Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res., Volume 1 (1993), pp. 219-222 | DOI

[6] S. Chieffi; A. Iavarone; A. Viggiano; M. Monda; S. Carlomagno Effect of a visual distractor on line bisection, Exp. Brain. Res., Volume 219 (2012), pp. 489-498 | DOI

[7] S. Chieffi; T. Iachini; A. Iavarone; G. Messina; A. Viggiano; M. Monda Flanker interference effects in a line bisection task, Exp. Brain. Res., Volume 232 (2014), pp. 1327-1334 | DOI

[8] S. Chieffi Visual illusion and line bisection: a bias hypothesis revisited, Exp. Brain. Res., Volume 234 (2016), pp. 1451-1458 | DOI

[9] S. Chieffi; A. Iavarone; L. Iaccarino; M. La Marra; G. Messina; V. De Luca et al. Age-related differences in distractor interference on line bisection, Exp. Brain Res., Volume 232 (2014), pp. 3659-3664 | DOI

[10] P.W. Halligan; L. Manning; J.C. Marshall Individual variation in line bisection: a study of four patients with right hemisphere damage and normal controls, Neuropsychologia, Volume 28 (1990), pp. 1043-1051

[11] L. Manning; P.W. Halligan; J.C. Marshall Individual variation in line bisection: a study of normal subjects with application to the interpretation of visual neglect, Neuropsychologia, Volume 28 (1990), pp. 647-655

[12] S. Chieffi; A. Iavarone; S. Carlomagno Effects of spatiotopic factors on bisection of radial lines, Exp. Brain Res., Volume 189 (2008), pp. 129-132 | DOI

[13] S. Chieffi; G. Messina; I. Villano; A. Messina; C.R. Ilardi; M. Monda et al. Hemispheric asymmetries in radial line bisection: role of retinotopic and spatiotopic factors, Front. Psychol., Volume 9 (2018), p. 2200 | DOI

[14] M. Drain; P.A. Reuter-Lorenz Vertical orienting control: evidence for attentional bias and “neglect” in the intact brain, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., Volume 125 (1996), pp. 139-158 | DOI

[15] C.M. Butter; J. Evans; N. Kirsch; D. Kewman Altitudinal neglect following traumatic brain injury: a case report, Cortex, Volume 25 (1989), pp. 135-146

[16] M. Mennemeier; E. Wertman; K.M. Heilman Neglect of near peripersonal space. Evidence for multidirectional attentional systems in humans, Brain, Volume 115 (1992), pp. 37-50 | DOI

[17] S. Chieffi; G. Messina; A. Messina; I. Villano; V. Monda; F.I. Ambra et al. Memory for spatial locations in a patient with near space neglect and optic ataxia: involvement of the occipitotemporal stream, Front. Neurol., Volume 8 (2017), p. 231 | DOI

[18] J.C. Adair; D.J. Williamson; D.H. Jacobs; D.L. Na; M. Heilman Neglect of radial and vertical space: importance of the retinotopic reference frame, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, Volume 58 (1995), pp. 724-728

[19] R.C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory, Neuropsychologia, Volume 9 (1971), pp. 97-113

[20] S. Ishiai; T. Furukawa; H. Tsukagoshi Visuospatial processes of line bisection and the mechanisms underlying unilateral spatial neglect, Brain, Volume 112 (1989), pp. 1485-1502

[21] P. Fraisse; S. Ehrlich; E. Vurpillot Études de la centration perceptive par la méthode tachistoscopique, Arch. Psychol., Volume 35 (1956), pp. 193-214

[22] S. Masin Test of the effect of attention on judged length of a line, Percept. Mot. Skills, Volume 96 (2003), pp. 599-606 | DOI

[23] D.A. Milner; M. Brechmann; L. Pagliarini To halve and to halve not: an analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects, Neuropsychologia, Volume 30 (1992), pp. 515-526 | DOI

[24] W. Prinzmetal; A. Wilson The effect of attention on phenomenal length, Perception, Volume 26 (1997), pp. 193-205 | DOI


Comments - Policy