Outline
Comptes Rendus

Ethology / Éthologie
Kin recognition versus familiarity in a solitary mustelid, the European polecat Mustela putorius
Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 331 (2008) no. 3, pp. 248-254.

Abstracts

The aim of this experimental study was to investigate kin discrimination in the polecat and to analyse the ontogeny of interactions. Juvenile polecats (ten males and nine females) had been raised under four distinct experimental conditions: 1, kin, familiar; 2, kin, unfamiliar; 3, non-kin, familiar; 4, non-kin, unfamiliar. During dyadic encounters between polecats in neutral enclosures, the number of positive (tolerance), negative (aggression), intermediate (intimidation), and neutral interactions (no direct interactions) were recorded at two different ages of the animals (50 and 70 days old). Male–male encounters were characterised by more aggressive behaviour than female–female ones. The proportion of these negative interactions increased with age, while the proportion of positive interactions decreased. Although aggressive behaviours varied among groups, the reaction did never differ with the kinship. Kin selection theory provides successful explanations for a wide range of phenomena, but our results suggest that multiple mechanisms running simultaneously might be involved in social behaviours. Familiarity clearly influenced the social behaviour of polecats and might be involved in a kin facilitation effect favouring interactions. Animals raised together demonstrated more positive and less negative interactions, so that, despite the individualistic way of life of the polecat, familiarisation may result in more tolerance, emphasising that solitary species may provide significant information on social life. Anyway, familiarisation in polecat may be regarded as a cognitive form of recognition.

L'objectif de cette recherche expérimentale est d'étudier la discrimination de la parentèle chez le putois et d'analyser l'ontogenèse des interactions. Des putois juvéniles (dix mâles et neuf femelles) ont été élevés dans quatre conditions expérimentales distinctes : 1, parents, familiers ; 2, parents, non-familiers ; 3 : non-apparentés, familiers ; 4, non-apparentés, non-familiers. Pendant les rencontres dyadiques entre les putois dans des enceintes neutres, le nombre d'interactions positives (tolérance), négatives (agression), intermédiaires (intimidation) et neutres (pas d'interactions directes) a été enregistré à deux âges différents (50 et 70 jours). Les rencontres entre mâles étaient caractérisées par un comportement plus agressif que celui des femelles. La proportion de ces interactions négatives augmentait avec l'âge tandis que la proportion d'interactions positives diminuait. Bien que les comportements agressifs varient selon les groupes, la réaction ne différait jamais avec la parenté. La théorie de sélection de parenté fournit des explications fructueuses pour nombre de phénomènes, mais nos résultats suggèrent que de multiples mécanismes se produisant simultanément pourraient être impliqués dans les comportements sociaux. La familiarité influence nettement le comportement social du putois et peut être impliquée dans un effet de facilitation de la parenté, favorisant des interactions. Les animaux élevés ensemble présentent plus d'interactions positives et moins d'interactions négatives, si bien que, et malgré le mode de vie individualiste du putois, la familiarisation pourrait entraîner plus de tolérance, soulignant que les espèces solitaires peuvent fournir des informations pertinentes sur la vie sociale. Dans tous les cas, la familiarisation chez le putois peut être considérée comme une forme cognitive de reconnaissance.

Metadata
Received:
Accepted:
Published online:
DOI: 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006
Keywords: Aggressive behaviour, Familiarity, Kin recognition, Ontogeny, Mustela putorius, Polecat
Mot clés : Comportement agressif, Familiarité, Reconnaissance de la parenté, Ontogenèse, Mustela putorius, Putois

Thierry Lodé 1

1 UMR CNRS 6552 Éthologie–Évolution–Écologie, université Rennes-1 & Université d'Angers, faculté des sciences, Belle Beille, F-49045 Angers cedex, France
@article{CRBIOL_2008__331_3_248_0,
     author = {Thierry Lod\'e},
     title = {Kin recognition versus familiarity in a solitary mustelid, the {European} polecat {\protect\emph{Mustela} putorius}},
     journal = {Comptes Rendus. Biologies},
     pages = {248--254},
     publisher = {Elsevier},
     volume = {331},
     number = {3},
     year = {2008},
     doi = {10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006},
     language = {en},
}
TY  - JOUR
AU  - Thierry Lodé
TI  - Kin recognition versus familiarity in a solitary mustelid, the European polecat Mustela putorius
JO  - Comptes Rendus. Biologies
PY  - 2008
SP  - 248
EP  - 254
VL  - 331
IS  - 3
PB  - Elsevier
DO  - 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006
LA  - en
ID  - CRBIOL_2008__331_3_248_0
ER  - 
%0 Journal Article
%A Thierry Lodé
%T Kin recognition versus familiarity in a solitary mustelid, the European polecat Mustela putorius
%J Comptes Rendus. Biologies
%D 2008
%P 248-254
%V 331
%N 3
%I Elsevier
%R 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006
%G en
%F CRBIOL_2008__331_3_248_0
Thierry Lodé. Kin recognition versus familiarity in a solitary mustelid, the European polecat Mustela putorius. Comptes Rendus. Biologies, Volume 331 (2008) no. 3, pp. 248-254. doi : 10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2007.12.006/

Version originale du texte intégral

1 Introduction

Kin recognition developed as a major research subject as soon as Hamilton (1963) introduced the notion of inclusive fitness leading to kin selection, i.e. an increasing fitness through the breeding of relatives [1,2]. The relationship between kin recognition (an internal process) and kin discrimination (observable kin bias in behaviour) is a complex one. First, although in many cases kin bias has been proved to be linked to recognition, kin bias does not necessarily involve kin recognition. Second, lack of kin discrimination does not imply a failure to recognise kin, which can be revealed only by appropriate experimentation [3].

Ability for individual discrimination has been demonstrated within numerous species [4–6]. Many studies have focused on the underlying mechanisms of kin discrimination. These mechanisms are diverse, but they can be divided into two main categories. Kin discrimination by conspecifics cues [7] occurs through the detection of phenotypic similarities in the absence of previous experience. When prior experiences are required, kin discrimination arises via direct or indirect familiarisation (self-matching or allo-matching) [8]. Indeed, only indirect familiarisation allows the animal to recognise an unfamiliar kin. However, as put forward by Waldam [9], the extent of mutual exclusion between some of these mechanisms remains far from clear.

Many functions of kin recognition have been described previously. These include care of offspring, helping siblings (allo-grooming, alarm…), cooperation, development of effective bonds, communal breeding, helpers at the nest [10,11], escaping from cannibalism [12], mate choice and avoidance of inbreeding [13,14]. Hamilton [15] also emphasised that kin selection theory could also be applied to aggressive behaviour. In the 1980s, studies on spiny mice [16], primates such as Macaca nemestria [17], ground squirrels [18], or golden hamsters [19] improved our understanding of kin recognition in mammals. Holmes and Sherman [18] demonstrated that sisters, in contrast to brothers, displayed less aggressive behaviour among themselves, even if they were separated at birth (unfamiliar kin), than when confronted with non-kin females.

Most studies on kin recognition have examined social species where individuals are linked by social bonds throughout their life. However, in many species, including mammals, bonds are not long lasting; they are limited to mother-offspring and sibling ties. Adult male–female relationships are often restricted to reproductive periods. In polecats (Mustela putorius), male and female territorial boundaries are defined by scent marking [20], which limits direct confrontation between individuals. This solitary or individualistic characteristic of the polecat [21,22] is expressed by aggressive encounters, including between males and females [23–26]. During reproductive periods, behavioural modifications lead to short-lived tolerance between male and female adults [26,27]. Despite their individualistic way of life, communal activities have been observed in some mustelids, including foraging and sharing of prey [28,29].

That solitary carnivores show mechanisms for kin discrimination may be addressed. Recently, Tang-Martinez [30] hypothesized whether kin discrimination may derive from other, non-specialized abilities of animals. Determining the mechanisms favouring recognition is hence a fundamental question. The issue is especially to distinguish kin versus familiarity effect. The aim of this research was first to develop an experimental design in order to detect kin discrimination in the polecat and to specify the mechanism underlying this discrimination. Second, I analysed the ontogeny of interactions at the time of active discovery of the environment, i.e. 50 days after birth and just before dispersal when they were 70 days old.

2 Methods

The study was carried out on five litters (a, b, c, d and e) of laboratory-bred polecats (10 males and 9 females, Authorisation DPN, ‘Direction de la protection de la Nature’ and Capacity Certificate). Litters ‘a’, ‘b’, ‘c’, and ‘d’ were identified by a coloured mark. Newborn animals were separated from their mothers 10 days after birth and divided into four groups:

  • – group 1, kin, familiar – related animals (siblings, i.e. brothers and sisters) were raised by their biological mother;
  • – group 2, kin, unfamiliar – related animals (siblings) were raised by two different ‘mothers’;
  • – group 3, non-kin, familiar – these animals were born of different parents, but raised together by the same unrelated ‘mother’;
  • – group 4, non-kin, unfamiliar – unrelated animals were raised by different ‘mothers’ – litter size varied from 3 to 5.

Fifty-four dyads were tested: 13 were male–male, 10 were female–female, and 31 were male–female. Animals of each dyad were introduced simultaneously into a 16-m2 neutral enclosure just at the dusk because of twilight and of the nocturnal habits of the species [31]; they were observed using a red light. Interactions were studied during these dyadic encounters that lasted 10 minutes, but some were interrupted before intense aggression was displayed. Between tests, animals were isolated from their real or adoptive mother and their littermates during two consecutive days.

The results of each encounter were classified into one of four behavioural categories. The first three were defined according to the degree of tolerance observed: (1) negative interactions characterised by more or less pronounced aggression, (2) intermediate interactions – displays of intimidating behaviour –, (3) positive interactions – tolerance, investigation of the other, play. As during some encounters, there were no direct interactions, we added a fourth category of behaviour: exploration of surroundings, self-grooming, etc. This last category was labelled non-interactive behaviour. Encounters between a given dyad were replicated five times. Confrontations were staged every other day.

Two types of data analysis were made. Firstly, individuals were tested as separate units (n=19). Secondly, each encounter was considered a unit and data are expressed as proportions of encounters in each behavioural category (n=54). A first series of dyadic encounters were carried out on polecats aged 48–55 days. A second series of dyadic encounters, following the same protocol, were carried out when the animals were 70 days old. Results were analysed by non-parametric statistical tests adapted to the type of data, taking into account related and independent values (H Kruskal–Wallis, U Mann–Whitney, τ Wilcoxon).

3 Results

A comparison between the four groups revealed differences, both for positive interactions or negative interactions (Table 1). For intermediate interactions, only female–female and male–female encounters showed significant variations among groups and only male–female encounters significantly varied among groups for non-interactive behaviour. This study aimed at detecting the contribution of four variables – sex, age, kinship, and familiarity – to these variations.

Table 1

Differences among types of interactions in the European polecat

Differences Male–male Female–female Male–female
Positive interactions KW = 22.6 KW = 16.7 KW = 49.2
p=0.0001 p=0.0001 p=0.0001
Negative interactions KW = 20.1 KW = 17.1 KW = 39.5
p=0.0001 p=0.0001 p=0.0001
Intermediate interactions KW = 2.12 KW = 13.1 KW = 16.8
Not significant p=0.004 p=0.001
Non-interactive behaviours KW = 4.69 KW = 1.02 KW = 12.4
Not significant Not significant p=0.01

3.1 Sex effect

Males were found more aggressive than females (U=16, Z=1.06, p<0.05). Analysis of male–male and female–female encounters showed that male–male encounters were characterised by more negative interactions (48.5%) than female–female ones (21%, U=166, Z=2.16, p=0.03). The contrary was true for non-interactive behaviour, which accounted for 14% of the female–female and 3.1% of the male–male encounters (U=129, Z=3.45, p=0.006). There were no significant differences between data for male–male and for female–female encounters for the other categories of interactions (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1

Sex effect. Respective proportions of different interactions in the European polecat depending on the sex of individuals.

3.2 Age effect

Male–male, female–female, and male–female encounters were analysed, yielding 270 encounters for each age series. Comparisons between data for the two age groups studied (i.e. 50 and 70 days) highlighted the fact that incidences of negative interactions tended to increase with age (Fig. 2, τ=11.5, p<0.01), and so did non-interactive behaviour (τ=69.5, p<0.01). In contrast, incidences of intermediate and positive interactive behaviour decreased significantly with age (τ=62.0, p<0.02; τ=20.0, p<0.01).

Fig. 2

Age effect. Respective proportions of different interactions in the European polecat depending on the age of individuals.

3.3 Kin effect

In groups 2 and 4, all animals were unfamiliar, but were either kin (group 2) or non-kin (group 4). Considering all types of encounters (male–male, male–female, female–female for both age series), the proportion of positive interactions between unfamiliar kin did not differ significantly from that observed between unfamiliar non-kin (U=346, Z=0.75, p>0.05). These results were not significant as well for male–male encounters, as for female–female or for male–female encounters (respectively, U=21.5, U=8, U=110, p>0.05). There were no differences in the proportion of negative interactions between unfamiliar kin and unfamiliar non-kin (U=322.5, Z=1.14, p>0.05). There were no significant differences regarding every category of encounters (male–male U=19, female–female U=5.5, male–female U=97.5, p>0.05).

In groups 1 and 3, all animals were familiar, but differed in kinship. No significant differences were noted between animals from groups 1 and 3 as regards the proportion either of negative or of positive interactions (respectively, U=312, Z=0.48, p>0.05 and U=329.5, Z=0.156, p>0.05). These findings showed no differences regarding every category of encounters (positive interactions: male–male U=14, p>0.05, female–female U=11, p>0.05, male–female U=100.5, p>0.05; negative interactions: male–male U=14, p>0.05, female–female U=12, p>0.05, male–female U=108, p>0.05).

As no significant kin effect could be evidenced, data were pooled for encounters between, on the one hand, familiar animals (groups 1 and 3), and, on the other hand, unfamiliar animals (groups 2 and 4). Therefore, data for interactions between familiar animals (groups 1 and 3) could be compared to those for interactions between unfamiliar animals (groups 2 and 4).

3.4 Familiarity effect

Familiarity influenced significantly the proportion of positive and negative interactions observed during male–male and female–female encounters (familiar/unfamiliar: U=0, p<0.002 for every encounter, Fig. 3).

Fig. 3

Familiarity effect. Respective proportions of different interactions in the European polecat depending on the familiarity of individuals.

Familiarity also influenced significantly the proportions of all four categories of interactions in male–female encounters (positive U=0.5, Z=7.01, p=0.0001, negative U=90, Z=6.14, p<0.0001, intermediate U=221, Z=3.99, p<0.0001, non-interactive U=266.5, Z=3.29, p<0.001).

Familiarity influenced significantly the proportion of intermediate interactions, but only in female–female encounters (U=6.5, p<0.002, Fig. 3).

4 Discussion

This study raised several issues for kin recognition.

  • (1) The reactions of females clearly differed from those of males, who were always more aggressive. This aggression has a functional significance in terms of reproductive strategy by favouring a male to assert his territoriality [32,33]. The lower level of aggressiveness in interactions between females could be conveyed by asserting territoriality less than males.
  • (2) Non-interactive behaviour as well as aggression increased with age, thereby reinforcing the polecat's individualistic tendency [25,27].
  • (3) Kinship did not influence significantly the behaviour of polecats when they were raised under similar conditions; sibling separated soon after birth behaved like separated non-kin. Therefore, there is no clear behavioural discrimination of kin in polecats, thus excluding the presence of any mechanism of kin recognition without prior experience. However, although they were not statistically significant, differences tend to emerge between related and unrelated groups concerning the proportion of positive and negative interactions, and the possibility that, under different conditions, kinship may influence interactions more cannot be ruled out.
  • (4) The behaviour of animals raised together clearly differed from that of animals raised separately. Animals raised together were more tolerant of each other in that they exhibited more positive interactions and less negative interactions compared to animals raised apart. There is therefore a familiarisation process that is not modulated, or only slightly, by kinship, since interactions between siblings raised together and those between non-siblings raised together did not differ significantly.

Young animals raised together learn to recognise one another; this recognition through prior experience implies familiarisation by allo-reference. This distinction between familiar versus unknown individual explains their subsequent behavioural discrimination (i.e. a particular behaviour according to the familiar/unknown status of the conspecifics encountered). Increasing the benefit of territoriality, individual recognition by familiarisation may allow reducing the intensity of agonistic encounters. The importance of experience acquired during the first encounters can be determining in mechanisms of kin recognition [34–36], and Taylor and Irwin [37] showed that altruism might be promoted by overlapping generations. Erhart et al. [38] suggested that social learning and social history are the most likely mechanisms for kin recognition. Other authors such as [39,40] or Aragon et al. [6] stressed that familiarisation mechanisms can play an important role in the social biology of a species, as does real kinship. Thus, coalition behaviour or cooperative reproduction (helpers at the nest) has been observed even in the absence of kinship, cooperative foraging being a key factor influencing social tolerance [29,41–44].

In social species, such as baboons [45] and vervets [46], unrelated animals show a sort of cooperation called ‘reciprocal altruism’. In the ferret, Mustela furo, scent molecules emitted by the anal gland differ between males and females [47]. Furthermore, differences in concentrations of constituents of these scents provide information about the identity of animals within a species, thereby allowing them to distinguish the sex of conspecifics and to find out whether or not they are familiar [47].

Mutual tolerance could be expressed at a very young age, thereby inhibiting, at least partially, antagonisms between animals that are familiar with one another. Because polecats' responses from kin versus non-kin did not differ, my results suggest that polecats referred to their own odour (self-referent phenotype matching). Analyzing kin recognition in golden hamsters, Heth et al. [19] and Mateo and Johnston [48] also concluded that self-referent phenotype matching is involved in specific recognition. Actually, some species such as baboons recognize kin only when they live in maternal association [38]. In polecat, familiarity may be used to recognise littermates, regardless they are kin or non-kin, and this discrimination may favour a kin facilitation effect for mate choice and territory acquisition in females or in competition in males. Juvenile polecats raised apart showed intolerance towards conspecifics during encounters. This aggressiveness is probably linked to the individualism of polecats [27]. A similar process was also evidenced in social species. In polycalic ants, the weak antagonism between neighbour colonies was attributed to their genetic relatedness [49], but Langlen et al. [50] argued that the decrease in aggressiveness mainly results from habituation effect acting as a ‘dear enemy’ effect [51]. The intraspecific aggression and the individualism may have an adaptive significance when animals compete for restricted resources. Because kin recognition allows one to discriminate between relatives [13,14], inbreeding avoidance is another significant benefit of specific recognition of littermates. This antagonism induces a territorial way of life: intrasexual territoriality accompanied by a temporary form of sexual segregation for habitat exploitation, females avoiding frequenting the same sites as males do [20,25]. Furthermore, since familiarity with conspecifics increases tolerance in polecats, one could expect that animals possessing close or adjoining territories would be more likely to come from the same litters. Thus, Allen and Sargeant [52] showed that red fox littermates tended to disperse in similar directions. This settling in close proximity would not be without consequences on the genetic structure and evolution of populations [2].

Kin selection theory provides successful explanations for a wide range of phenomena, but my results suggest that multiple mechanisms running simultaneously might be involved in social behaviours. It may be argued that recognition is chiefly based on familiarisation rather than constituting the evolution of a specialized kin recognition system. Familiarisation in polecat may act as a cognitive form of recognition supporting the Tang-Martinez's conjecture [30], asserting that kin discrimination results from an extension of other, non-specialized sensory and cognitive abilities of animals. Anyway, tolerance through familiarisation could be expressed even in a species where, until now, the individualistic character of animals has been emphasised, thus underlining that solitary species may provide significant information on social life.


References

[1] J. Maynard Smith Group selection and kin selection, Nature, Volume 201 (1964), pp. 1145-1147

[2] A.S. Griffin; S.A. West Kin selection: Fact and fiction, Trends Ecol. Evol., Volume 17 (2002), pp. 15-21

[3] P. Hepper Sibling recognition in the rat, Anim. Behav., Volume 31 (1987), pp. 1177-1191

[4] Z.T. Halpin Individual odors among mammals: Origins and functions, Adv. Stud. Behav., Volume 16 (1986), pp. 39-70

[5] D.P. Whitfield Plumage variability, status signalling and individual recognition in avian flocks, Trends Ecol. Evol., Volume 2 (1987), pp. 13-18

[6] P. Aragón; P. López; J. Martín Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: Implications of field spatial relationships between males, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 50 (2001), pp. 128-133

[7] C.J. Barnard Kin recognition: Problems, prospects, and the evolution of discrimination systems, Adv. Stud. Behav., Volume 19 (1990), pp. 29-81

[8] P.J.B. Slater Kinship and altruism (P.J.B. Slater; T.R. Halliday, eds.), Behaviour and Evolution, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1994, pp. 193-222

[9] B. Waldam Mechanisms of kin recognition, J. Theor. Biol., Volume 128 (1987), pp. 159-185

[10] G.S. Wilkinson; A.E.M. Baker Communal nesting among genetically similar house mice, Ethology, Volume 77 (1988), pp. 103-114

[11] A. Cockburn Evolution of helping behavior in cooperatively breeding birds, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., Volume 29 (1998), pp. 141-177

[12] D.W. Pfennig; J.P. Collins; R.E. Ziemba A test of alternative hypotheses for kin recognition in cannibalistic tiger salamanders, Behav. Ecol., Volume 10 (1999), pp. 436-443

[13] P.P.G. Bateson Optimal outbreeding (P.P.G. Bateson, ed.), Mate Choice, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1983, pp. 257-277

[14] D.W. Pfennig Kin recognition (M. Pagel, ed.), Encyclopedia of Evolution, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2002, pp. 592-595

[15] W.D. Hamilton The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I & II, J. Theor. Biol., Volume 7 (1964), pp. 1-52

[16] R.H. Porter; V.J. Tepper; D.J. White Experiential influences on the development of huddling preferences and “sibling” recognition in spiny mice, Dev. Psychobiol., Volume 14 (1981), pp. 375-382

[17] H.M.H. Wu; W.G. Holmes; S.R. Medina; G.P. Sackett Kin preference in infant Macaca nemestrina, Nature, Volume 285 (1980), pp. 225-227

[18] W.G. Holmes; P.W. Sherman The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels, Am. Zool., Volume 22 (1982), pp. 491-517

[19] G. Heth; J. Todrank; R.E. Johnston Kin recognition in golden hamsters: Evidence for phenotype matching, Anim. Behav., Volume 56 (1998), pp. 409-417

[20] B.K. Clapperton Scent-marking behaviour of the ferret, Mustela furo L, Anim. Behav., Volume 38 (1989), pp. 436-446

[21] T. Lodé Time budget as related to feeding tactics of European polecat Mustela putorius, Behav. Process., Volume 47 (1999), pp. 11-18

[22] T.B. Poole Aspects of aggressive behaviour in polecats, Z. Tierpsychol., Volume 24 (1967), pp. 351-369

[23] T.B. Poole Diadic interactions between pairs of male polecats (Mustela furo and Mustela furo × M. putorius hybrids), Z. Tierpsychol., Volume 30 (1972), pp. 45-58

[24] T.B. Poole The effects of oestrous condition and familiarity on the sexual behaviour of polecats (Mustela putorius and M. furo × M. putorius hybrids), J. Zool. Lond., Volume 172 (1974), pp. 357-362

[25] T. Lodé Conspecific tolerance and sexual segregation in the use of space and habitats in the European polecat, Acta Theriol., Volume 41 (1996), pp. 171-176

[26] T. Lodé Mating system and genetic variance in a polygynous mustelid, the European polecat, Genes Gen. Syst., Volume 76 (2001), pp. 221-227

[27] T. Lodé; R. Berzins; V. Pereboom Implications of an individualistic lifestyle for species conservation: Lessons from jealous beasts, C. R. Biologies, Volume 326 (2003), p. S30-S36

[28] E. Pulliainen Use of the home range by pine martens (Martes martes L.), Acta Zool. Fenn., Volume 171 (1984), pp. 271-274

[29] G.M. Blundell; M. Ben-David; P. Groves; R.T. Bowyers; E. Geffen Characteristics of sex-biased dispersal and gene flow in coastal river otters: Implications for natural recolonization of extirpated populations, Mol. Ecol., Volume 11 (2002), pp. 289-303

[30] Z. Tang-Martinez The mechanisms of kin discrimination and the evolution of kin recognition in vertebrates: A critical re-evaluation, Behav. Proc., Volume 53 (2001), pp. 21-40

[31] T. Lodé Activity pattern of polecats Mustela putorius L. in relation to food habits and prey activity, Ethology, Volume 100 (1995), pp. 295-308

[32] T. Lodé Functional response and area-restricted search of a predator: Seasonal exploitation of anurans by European polecat Mustela putorius, Austral Ecol., Volume 25 (2000), pp. 223-231

[33] R.A. Powell Mustelid spacing patterns: Variations on a theme by Mustela, Z. Tierpsychol., Volume 50 (1979), pp. 153-165

[34] A.M. Kareem; C.J. Barnard The importance of kinship and familiarity in social interactions between mice, Anim. Behav., Volume 30 (1982), pp. 594-601

[35] I. Blais; J. Terkel; A. Goldblatt Long-term impact of early olfactory experience on later olfactory conditioning, Dev. Psychobiol., Volume 48 (2006), pp. 501-507

[36] J.M. Mateo Development of individually distinct recognition cues, Dev. Psychobiol., Volume 48 (2006), pp. 508-519

[37] P.D. Taylor; A.J. Irwin Overlapping generations can promote altruistic behaviour, Evolution, Volume 54 (2000), pp. 1135-1141

[38] E.M. Erhart; A.M. Coelho; C.A. Bramblett Kin Recognition by Paternal Half-Siblings in Captive Papio cynocephalus, Am. J. Primatol., Volume 43 (1997), pp. 147-157

[39] A.R. Blaustein; R.K. O'Hara Genetic control for sibling recognition, Nature, Volume 290 (1981), pp. 246-248

[40] P.O. Hopkins Les animaux reconnaissent-ils leurs parents?, La Recherche, Volume 148 (1983), pp. 1296-1297

[41] J.D. Ligon; S.H. Ligon Communal breeding in green woodhoopoes as a case for reciprocity, Nature, Volume 276 (1978), pp. 496-498

[42] C. Packer; A.E. Pusey Cooperation and competition within coalitions of male lions: Kin selection or game theory?, Nature, Volume 296 (1982), pp. 740-742

[43] H. Rita; E. Ranta; P. Nina Competition in foraging groups, Oikos, Volume 76 (1996), pp. 583-586

[44] R.D. Magrath; L.A. Whittingham Subordinate males are more likely to help if unrelated to the breeding female in cooperatively breeding white-browed scrubwrens, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 41 (1997), pp. 185-192

[45] C. Packer Reciprocal altruism in Papio anubis, Nature, Volume 265 (1977), pp. 441-443

[46] R.M. Seyfarth; D.L. Cheney Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys, Nature, Volume 308 (1984), pp. 541-542

[47] B.K. Clapperton; E.O. Minot; D.R. Crump An olfactory recognition in the ferret Mustela furo L. (Carnivora: Mustelidae), Anim. Behav., Volume 36 (1988), pp. 541-553

[48] J.M. Mateo; R.E. Johnston Kin recognition and the ‘armpit effect’: Evidence of self-referent phenotype matching, Proc. R. Soc. Lond., Ser. B, Volume 267 (2000), pp. 695-700

[49] M. Beye; P. Neumann; M. Chapuisat; P. Pamilo; R.F.A. Moritz Nestmate recognition and the genetic relatedness of nests in the an Formica pratensis, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 43 (1998), pp. 67-72

[50] T.A. Langlen; F. Tripet; P. Nomacs The red and the black: Habituation and the dear-enemy phenomenon in two desert Pheidole ants, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., Volume 48 (2000), pp. 285-292

[51] R.J. Brooks; J.B. Falls Individual recognition by song in white-throated sparrows. I. Discrimination of songs of neighbors and strangers, Can. J. Zool., Volume 53 (1975), pp. 879-888

[52] S.H. Allen; A.B. Sargeant Dispersal patterns of red foxes relative to population density, J. Wildl. Manage., Volume 57 (1993), pp. 526-533


Comments - Politique